Skip to main content

Traditional knowledge hiding in plain sight – twenty-first century ethnobotany of the Chácobo in Beni, Bolivia

Abstract

Background

The Chácobo are a Panoan speaking tribe of about 1000 members (300+ adults) in Beni, Bolivia. Originally nomadic, the Chácabo were relocated to their current main location in the 1960s. Researchers have visited the Chácabo since 1911. A first more detailed anthropological report exists from the late 1960s, and ecological–ethnobotanical studies were conducted in the 1980s and 1990s. The presented work represents a complete ethnobotanical inventory of the entire adult Chácobo population, with interviews and plant collection conducted directly by Chácobo counterparts.

Methods

Based on previous reports and our preliminary studies, we hypothesized that twenty-first century Chácobo plant use centered on income generation, and that traditional plant use related to household utensils, medicine and traditional crop varieties had almost disappeared. To test this hypothesis, we started the “Chácobo Ethnobotany Project,” training 10 indigenous Chácobo participants in ethnobotanical interview and plant collection techniques, in order to more fully document Chácobo knowledge and avoid the influence of foreign interviewers.

Results

Our study found 331 useful plant species in 241genera of 95 plant families, with leaves, roots and bark being the most commonly used plant parts The comprehensive documentation that these methods enabled completely nullified our initial hypothesis of knowledge loss. Traditional crop varieties are still widely grown and traditional knowledge is alive. Moreover, it is being actively recuperated in certain domains by the younger generation. Most Chácobo know, and can name, traditional utensils and tools, although only the older generation has still the skills to manufacture them. While many Chácobo still know the names and uses of medicinal species, the younger generation is however often unsure how to identify them.

Conclusions

In this paper we illustrate the complexity of perspectives on knowledge at different ages, and the persistence of knowledge over almost a century. We found that traditional knowledge was only partially affected by the processes of exposure to a market economy, and that different knowledge domains experienced different trends as a result of these changes. Overall knowledge was widely distributed, and we did not observe a directional knowledge loss.

We stress the importance to not directly conclude processes of knowledge loss, cultural erosion or acculturation when comparing the knowledge of different age groups.

Background

The Chácobo tribe, living in Northeastern Bolivia, were first visited by the European traveler Erland Nordenskjöld in 1911 [1], followed by an anthropologist only in 1956, who published the last account of Chácobo life before the tribe came under the influence of American Evangelist missionaries [2]. The Summer Institute of Linguistics (SIL) worked with Chácobo communities from 1953 to 1980, and produced the first account of Chácobo linguistics [3], and an unpublished work on Chácobo customs, with a strong focus on evangelist development [4]. This account is in interesting juxtaposition to the writings of German anthropologist Kelm [5], who visited the Chácobo in 1970, in the middle of SIL rule. The SIL finally was replaced in 1980 by the Swiss Protestant mission. Missionary rule led to a profound change of lifestyle, and a permanent process of acculturation [6]. From 1983 to 84, Brian Boom (New York Botanical Garden) led the first ethnobotanical study of Chácobo, documenting their knowledge after almost 30 years of cultural change [7]. Boom did however base his work on the plants collected from a single 1 ha forest plot. In 1995 the Institut Franjáis d’Etudes Andines financed a re–survey of Boom’s plot, but the results were never released to the public, and a planned publication [8] existed in a single volume in the Institute’s main library in Lima. Muñoz et al. [9] published a study on anti–malarial plants used by the Chácobo. Given the availability of previous studies, the Chácobo are an outstanding possibility to study traditional knowledge over time.

Traditional knowledge (TK) has been recognized for its importance for the protection of ecosystem services and biodiversity [10, 11]. However, researchers and policymakers have equally expressed concern about its possible loss as societies modernize. A growing number of studies have reported changes and losses in TK (e.g. medical TK [12,13,14,15], nutritional TK [16], and agricultural TK [17,18,19]. The hypothesis that TK systems are able to adapt to external changes and internal pressures has discussed for some time (e.g., [20]). Traditional.

Knowledge is an important part of the adaptive capacity of many rural and indigenous communities that have been conserving biodiversity while enhancing livelihoods and adapting to disturbance and change [21, 22]. Few studies have however examined whether and how loss or alteration of TK in fact occurs [17, 23,24,25]. Consequently, our understanding of the resilience of TK systems and their ability to evolve and adapt is highly limited. The assumption of loss of TK, when younger people know less, is one of the common errors in ethnobotany [26]. This problem can be remedied when analyzing the effects of age and age cohorts separately [27, 28]. Not all TK domains might however be shared between generations, and as such there can be domains that would be more vulnerable to TK loss, and domains in which new knowledge is generated as an adaptation to environmental change [29].

Traditional knowledge is also seen as an important component in improving the management of natural resources [10, 20, 30] and practices relating to the protection of ecosystems and species [11]. Factors such as gender, age, ethnicity, birthplace, and level of education have been identified as important on an individual level [31,32,33,34]. Family size, integration into the market economy (e.g. sale of animals and agricultural products), or amount of material goods at family level (e.g., possessions of farm animals, tools, and transport) have been linked to the household levels [35, 36]. Access to commercial centers, and to health, education, electricity or water, as well as land tenure systems and settlement history have shown a greater relevance at the community level [37,38,39]. In the absence of a unifying theory or common research methods, it is however difficult to clearly recognize whether or not these patterns exist at broader scales [40]. Several studies have used literature metadata to analyze large–scale usage patterns of plants [41,42,43]. In many cases, however, comparisons are difficult to make, given the diversity of the objectives and methods employed.

Based on previous reports mentioned, and our own preliminary studies [44], we hypothesized that twenty-first century Chácobo plant use centered on income generation through collection of forest products and agricultural production, and that traditional plant use related to household artifacts and medicine, as well as traditional crop varieties, had almost disappeared. We also hypothesized that the “missionary generation” – the first age group growing up under restrictive evangelist rule, would report less TK than other age groups. Because access to markets and services has been reported as a major cause for TK loss [28], we also hypothesized that in villages most distant from the main market center (Riberalta), knowledge about the use of plants, and the number of useful species would be more homogeneously distributed through the generations [25], and expected that this TK distribution show different patterns when analyzing the different domains of knowledge about the use of plants [29].

To test our hypotheses, we started the “Chácobo Ethnobotany Project,” training 10 indigenous Chácobo participants in ethnobotanical interview and plant collection techniques, to comprehensively document contemporary Chácobo TK and avoid the limiting influence of foreign interviewers.

Methods

The study area –– The Chácobo and Pacahuara

The Chácobo belong to the Panoan linguistic group, which includes about twelve tribes (Chácobo, Pacahuara, Matis, Matses, Yaminahua, Ese Eja and others). At the end of the 1890s, the Chácobo lived as semi–nomadic hunters and cassava and maize cultivators, probably in two groups, one with six families and one with four, in north Bolivia, between Lake Roguagnado and the river Mamore, south of their current territory. During the rubber boom in the early 1900s, they were forced by more aggressive tribes to move north, where rubber tappers, who also brought disease and epidemics to the tribe, threatened them. While other tribes were enslaved to work in rubber stations, the Chácobo managed to avoid most of the outside influences. Their first permanent contact with the outside world occurred only in 1953 with members of the the Tribes Missions, and in 1954 the Bolivian government established an agency about 15 km from the current location of Puerto Limones. The missionary linguist Gilbert Prost arrived in 1955 under the auspices of the Summer Institute of Linguistics (SIL). According to [4] there were four Chácobo groups living between the Benicito and Yata rivers at that time, numbering about 200 people [7]. Prost and his wife continued to live among the Chácobo until 1980. In addition to translating the New Testament into Chácobo, they made some observations on cultural and linguistic practices [3, 4]. In 1964, Prost managed to buy a territory in the north of the Chácobo’s ancestral lands, forming the community of Alto Ivón, and most of the remaining population moved there. In 1965, the Bolivian government finally assigned 43,000 ha of land to the Chácobo, although this area was less than 10% of their original territory. The influence of the SIL caused profound cultural change among the Chácobo, including the reported abandonment of traditional costume and dances in 1969 [4].

The official indigenous organization of the Chácobo (Central Indígena de la Región Amazónica de Bolivia (CIRABO) estimates a current population of the Chácobo community of about 1000 people (350+ adults), with Alto Ivón as the largest settlement. The current territory of the tribe according to CIRABO encompasses 450,000 ha, and is roughly equivalent to the original extent of the tribe’s ancestral lands (Fig. 1). The elevation of the territory is about 200 m, and much of the vegetation can be classified as humid tropical Amazon rainforest. However, the territory encompasses also large tracts of periodically inundated savannas, dominated by Mauritiella armata, and large, drier, savanna areas with forest islands. The average annual temperature is 26.8 °C, with an average annual rainfall of 1560 mm. A distinct dry season lasts from June to November [7]. Today the Chácobo are governed by two indigenous organizations: The Capitanía Mayor Chácobo, closely linked to the evangelists, and the Chácobo– Pacahuara Association, recognized by the Central Indígena de la Región Amazónica de Bolivia (CIRABO), and supported by the Central de Pueblos Indigenas del Beni (CPIB) and the Confederacion de Pueblos Indigenas de Bolivia (CIDOB).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Chácobo territory 2013

Ethnobotanical and botanical collection

Our project explored the current traditional knowledge (TK) on plant use of the Chácobo and Pacahuara in Beni, Bolivia and had three goals: 1) to discover and document current traditional plant knowledge through interviews and surveys, 2) to inventory the current flora of the region, and 3) to repatriate the acquired knowledge as well as previous data to the community.

After obtaining consent from CIRABO, and before starting fieldwork, we conducted a community meeting in May 2013, involving representatives of all 27 villages in the Chácobo Territory, in order to obtain prior informed consent from all communities. This session included the repatriation of the results of previous studies [45,46,47]. In addition, during the project all available material on Chácobo plant use was translated to Spanish and repatriated [48]. The Chácobo community itself choose 12 local counterparts to be trained as ethnobotanical interviewers and plant collectors. In September 2013 we conducted a two–week workshop on ethnobiological methods and plant collection, training the 12 selected counterparts, 10 of which finally acted as interveiwers. Training was conducted directly in the field in the central village of Alto Ivón, and involved theoretical exercises (overview on methodology of interviews, collection and herbarium techniques), as well as extensive practical exercises (structuring and testing of questionnaires, test interviews among the participants, field interviews with local community members, plant collection in the field, preparation of herbarium specimens, plant and artifact collection in the local community, data–basing, and initial data analysis).

From November 2013 to May 2015, Chácobo interviewers collected ethnobotanical information from 301 Chácobo participants (150 women, 151 men, representing almost the entire adult Chácobo population), and over 1500 plant samples were collected. Prior to starting the interviews, every interviewer obtained prior oral informed consent from each participant. Chácobo participants were divided into five age classes (18–30 years old: 58 men, 52 women; 31–40 years old: 31 men, 36 women; 41–50 years old: 35 men, 36 women; 51–60 years old: 15 men, 7 women; and >60 years old: 12 men, 19 women). Because the study attempted to interview the whole adult Chácobo population, there was originally no emphasis on achieving a balanced age or gender distribution. All interviews were conducted at the homes of the participants by asking participants to freelist their plant knowledge following [49]. All plant uses were categorized following [49]. All interviews were preferably conducted in Chácobo. In a few cases where participants were not fully fluent in Chácobo, interviewers used Spanish as common language. The plant material was collected under permission from the Ministry of Environment and Water of the Plurinational State of Bolivia, and was identified and deposited at the National Herbarium of Bolivia (LPB) under the collection numbers of the Chácobo collectors. Nomenclature follows www.TROPICOS.org. Use descriptions were coded after the fact into subcategories and, for some analyses, into six major categories: fodder, fuel, medical, cultural, construction, tool, and food.

All work was carried out following the International Society for Ethnobiology Code of Ethics [50], and under the framework provided by the Nagoya Protocol on Access to Genetic Resources and Fair and equitable sharing of benefits arising from their use of the Convention on Biological Diversity, the Chácobo community retains the copyright of the traditional knowledge of all informants. Any commercial use of any of the information requires prior consensus with informants and communities, and an agreement on the distribution of benefits.

Data analysis

The total number of unique species reported and unique uses reported for each use category were compared across communities, genders, and age groups (16–30, 31–40, 41–50, 51–60, and 61–82) for 292 informants (dropping for this analysis 8 informants for whom age was not indicated).

To gain a more nuanced look at how these qualities affected not only the number of reports but which species or uses were reported, we ordered informants using non–metric multi–dimensional scaling on distance matrices for plants and uses, and tested how well vectors (age) and factors (gender, ethnicity, community) fit the location of informants in the ordination, using the R package vegan [51]. We used similar methods with plant family fit onto an ordination from distance matrices of plant–use combinations to test whether plant family explains the uses to which plants are put.

We used Indicator Value [52], as implemented in the R package labdsv [53] to combine occurrence frequency and mean abundance of species and uses to elucidate species and uses that had higher fidelity to and/or relative abundance in certain age groups or genders. For this analysis, the P value is the probability of finding an equally high indicator value in random permutations. Species with significantly high indicator values had higher fidelity and relative abundance in certain age groups / genders (were ‘indicators’). We further compared age and gender groups by informant consensus factor (ICF) for each use category, calculated as the number of use reports minus the number of taxa over the number of use reports minus one: (Nur − Nt)/(Nur − 1). We also measured consensus on species uses by quantifying what proportion of each species’ mentions fall within a specific use category.

Plant species and plant family importance was ranked by four metrics: Community and Informant Cultural Importance (CIcom/CIinf) — the sum within species across all plant–uses of the number of informants (for CIinf) or communities (for CIcom) reporting a plant–use over the number of informants/communities reporting the plant; Diversity of Uses (Du) — the Shannon Index of uses [51]; and Use Value (UV), the number of reports of a species over total number of informants asked in a region [54].

To test whether greater knowledge of Chácobo language was associated with a more similar set of knowledge and / or a larger knowledge set of plants and uses, we used the ordination based on uses to examine whether interviewees who reported more Chácobo names tended to report a more similar set of uses, and used linear regression to test whether the number of Chácobo names reported was significantly greater for those who reported more species or more uses.

Results

The availability of previous field data gives the unique opportunity to study the long–term change in knowledge of an indigenous group in the age of globalization. Our study found 331 useful plant species in 241genera of 95 plant families, with leaves, roots and bark being the most commonly used plant parts (Table 1).

Table 1 Plant species used by the Chácobo

The larger Chácobo communities showed very similar patterns in the number of species used, with differences within communities usually greater than between, although Nueva Unión stood out in reporting more food species (Fig 2a). Likewise, all communities were similar in plant–uses (use descriptions for a species within each use category), although in this case Nueva Unión reported fewer use descriptions within the Utensils and tools and Cultural categories, while Motacuzal and Alto Ivón reported more medical uses (Fig. 2b). Within these categories, number of species and uses was fairly consistent across age groups, though we observed a trend for some categories of more species and uses known with increasing age. The age group between 51 and 60 years (i.e. the first age group growing up under missionary rule), showed a slightly lower knowledge, especially evident in the medical and cultural categories but also in food plants (Fig. 3). These metrics are also quite similar across gender, although across most categories the average number of species and uses reported by women was slightly higher (Fig. 4).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Number of species (a) and uses (b) reported per interview reported in each use category for each community (N: Motacuzal = 25, Yato Benicito = 51, Alto Ivón = 83, Las Limas = 45, Las Petas = 15, Nueva Unión = 20, Puerto Tujuré = 11. 40 interviews without a community indicated are not shown)

Fig. 3
figure 3

Number of uses (a) and species (b) reported per interview reported in each use category for each age group (N: 16–30 = 110, 31–40 = 65, 41–50 = 68, 51–60 = 25, 61–81 = 24)

Fig. 4
figure 4

Number of uses (a) and species (b) reported per interview in each use category for men (N = 154) and women (N = 138)

Who uses what and how?

Despite the similarities among communities in total species and uses reported, we found that informant community significantly influenced both which plants and which uses individual informants reported (Table 2 a&b). In contrast, and in accord with the results above, age and gender did not significantly influence either. Ethnicity of the participants influenced which plant species they used, but did not explain what they were used for. Given the very low r2 values, it is clear that much variety in uses was not explained by any demographic and environmental variables explored (Table 2 a&b). In the ordination, we can see this effect more clearly: although there was much overlap, the communities clearly structure which plants were reported. This difference was however much driven by the reports Nueva Unión (Fig. 5).

Table 2 Who uses what, and how?
Fig. 5
figure 5

Reports by community for plant species (a) and uses (b)

While age did not in itself explain the ordination well, we did find certain plants to be associated with age categories. In this we found no indicator plants or uses among the first three age groups (16–30, 31–40, 41–50), which suggests to some extent that plants and uses reported by these groups are less distinct than that of the second two age groups (51–60, >60). The 51–60 age group was associated with by Styrax sp., Iryanthera juruensis, Xylopia ligustrifolia, Hirtella pilosissima, Inga sp. 1, and Piper nigrispicum, while the >60 group was indicated by Gustavia hexapetala, Astrocaryum aculeatum, Phenakospermum guianensis, Attalea phalerata, Apuleia leiocarpa, Bixa orellana, Hancornia speciosa, Zingiber officinale, and Eriotheca sp. Likewise, the use subcategory Firewood was associated with the 51–60 age group while the medicinal use subcategories: Skin and subcutaneous tissue, Sensory system, Respiratory system and Musculo–skeletal system all were associated with the >60 age group.

Likewise, although gender did not fit to the overall ordinations, there was a large number of plants associated with female, and a much smaller one with male respondents (Table 3).

Table 3 Indicator values of species by Gender

Interestingly, all indicator uses were exclusively associated with women (Table 4).

Table 4 Indicator uses and gender association

Informant consensus factors (ICF)

Looking at specific use categories we found broadly similar trends across age categories and genders: tool, construction and food uses usually had the most use reports. We found a lower number of medicinal use reports, although the same number of respondents reported medicinal uses. Food uses consistently had less ICF than tool and construction uses, and medicinal uses even less. Cultural uses, while often reported by fewer informants and with fewer uses, show disproportionately high ICF (Table 5).

Table 5 Who uses what, and how: informant consensus factor

Plant relative importance metrics did show a different picture underlining the problems of using diversity indices. The Cultural Importance Index yielded wildly different species sets for Community and Individuals, and both Use Value Index and Use–diversity Index again yielded different sets as species as most important (Table 6).

Table 6 Plant importance metrics

Because the Cultural Importance Index tends to prioritize species with few informants, we highlighted the species that had both high index values in general, and also a large number of reports to elucidate species that were of high importance in all indices. As result, Vismia macrophylla, Xylopia peruviana, Attalea phalerata, Gossypium barbadense, Attalea maripa and Phenakospermum guianensis were elucidated as the most important species in the daily life of the Chácobo community (Fig. 6). Overall, however, informant consensus was very high in across all age groups and across all use categories (Fig. 7). Arecaceae, Fabaceae, Malvaceae and Rubiaceae were found to be the most important plant families used across most indices, although Moraceae did yield a higher ranking in Use Value (Table 7).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Species sized by number of reports (Npr) plotted against four metrics of importance: Community Cultural Importance (a), Informant Cultural Importance (b), Use Value (c), and Diversity of Uses (d). The most important species (top right quadrant) are labelled with their names, and the six shared across all four metrics are numbered

Fig. 7
figure 7

Informant consensus factor (ICF) for each use category, among male and female respondants of each age group. Points are sized by the number of use reports (UR) per respondent

Table 7 What is used? (plant relative importance metrics by family)

Results also indicated that qualities of plants did to a certain extent explain which uses they were put to. A large number of plant families had specifically Medicinal uses, while other sets of plant families were specifically used for Food, Utensils and tools, and Construction. Not surprisingly, data also revealed that plant families with high importance in all indices calculated (Arecaceae, Fabaceae, Malvaceae and Rubiaceae) had uses in all categories (Fig. 8).

Fig. 8
figure 8

Assemblage of by plant family (r–squared = 0.39, p = 0.001)

Different use categories also had different levels of fidelity in the species that were reported for them. For instance, relatively few mentions in the construction and tool categories were of species that are uniquely associated with those categories. In contrast, a much greater proportion of mentions for medical uses were of species that were only used for medical uses. This pattern was also true of food plants (Fig. 9).

Fig. 9
figure 9

Number of mentions that of a species plotted against that species’ fidelity to the given use category

Does language influence use knowledge?

Interviewees who reported more Chácobo names did indeed tend to report more similar sets of knowledge, and knew more species and uses (Fig. 10). In addition, the number of plants or number of uses reported strongly increased with the number of Chácobo names participants knew (Fig. 11). Although in some degree this was a feature of the study (there was no way to informants to report more names than species), it was clear that very few of those participants with great knowledge of species or uses failed to report a large number of Chácobo names.

Fig. 10
figure 10

Chácobo language proficiency (point size) plotted onto the ordination of informants in plant space

Fig. 11
figure 11

The number of species (a) and uses (b) was significantly greater for those who reported more Chácobo names. Loess regression line (for Chácobo informants only) is shown

Discussion

While other studies found indecisive patterns of the influence of age, or accessibility to markets on traditional knowledge (negative [17, 23, 44, 55]; positive [25, 56, 57]), our study did not reveal any pattern that would link differences in plant–use knowledge to age or accessibility of a location, but simply to specific location and associated flora in each of the communities. In most communities the contact with nature still remains vital to the acquisition of knowledge [58, 59], and the facility to observe and identify the useful plants clearly adds to this.

The observation that local and indigenous languages often package rich traditional ecological knowledge has led to the question in many studies of whether indigenous language abilities influence plant knowledge, i.e. if native language speakers have a higher knowledge than participants only speaking a mainstream language [44, 60]. In our study, the link between language proficiency and other metrics of traditional knowledge (plants and uses reported) does support at least the correlation of these variables, and suggest the possibility of simultaneous language and knowledge retention (or erosion).

The general trend found in relation to the difference in intergenerational knowledge suggests that any patterns are most likely a result of both knowledge transmission, as well as in situ learning, and be related to the time during which people acquire and use knowledge, with the older informants taking more responsibility in their households, who have a need to learn and apply their knowledge [27, 54, 61]. The knowledge of older people might not have been affected by the need to find new subsistence activities, and was thus preserved without external influence [62]. The fact that the only generation that did show decrease of traditional knowledge (albeit slight) was the generation of 41–50-year old participants, who had grown up under restrictive missionary rule, is noteworthy.

The hypothesis that people who are relatively isolated from the market economy share more traditional knowledge than people who live close to cities or larger towns [25], was not met in our study, because in most places the contact to nature still remains vital to the acquisition of knowledge [58, 59]. The predominance of the use for Human food in the more widely shared knowledge can be explained as a long and constant learning process that begins in early childhood, and is common in the more remote locations [26, 54].

There is no doubt that Chácobo daily life has changed in the course of the last century. Early accounts of the Chácobo all indicate the wide use of bark–cloth, and little enthusiasm for the rather conservative clothing style which missionaries tried to introduce [2, 5]. Boom [7] mentions however the complete disappearance of this custom. However, while the Chácobo use western style clothing available in the markets of Riberalta, traditional bark cloth is still widely used for cultural purposes, and most participants knew how to make it.

Changes in the use of traditional implements were very subtle. Most households still use large pounding tubs, as well as the large wooden boards used to pound food, which have not changed over time. Large clay pans for roasting jibe (Manihot flour), and smaller ceramic pots are also widely used. Even little stools from the petioles of Mauritia flexuosa and balsa wood (Ochroma sp.), first documented by Nordenskjöld [1] are still found in many houses, although they were completely missed in all previous studies. The production of burden baskets has not changed since [1], and the same species are still used today. However, only a few older women in the communities still have the skills to weave baskets, and modern implements like backpacks are clearly replacing traditional materials. Similarly, canoes are still an important means of transportation. However, while Nordenskjöld, Haenke and Kelm described canoes made from bark [1, 2, 5], the modern variety is made of hollowed out tree trunks, which is already indicated in [7]. House construction and roofing have however not changed much in the last 100 years. Bows and arrows are still maintained as hunting implements, especially for fishing, and all arrow types found in previous studies are still used among the population, although 22 caliber rifles and 20 gauge shotguns are favored for hunting.

Based on previous reports, we originally hypothesized that many household artifacts as well as traditional clothing had disappeared from Chácobo life. Many of these artifacts were mentioned in the 1922–1970 accounts, but not in later studies. Boom [7] and Bergeron [8] in particular indicate that traditional tools and clothing had disappeared. This turned out to be an interview artifact. Early anthropologists, who focused on Chácobo daily life [2, 5], while Boom and Bergeron focused only on plants collected from one 1 ha forest plot [7, 8]. Our combined study indicates that in fact most artifacts of the Chácobo are still known, and also used, by a large part of the population. This includes traditional clothing that is still being prepared and used on important occasions, as well as hunting and household implements. In daily life however, no traditional clothing and ornaments are found anymore, and the large monkey tooth breast–plates mentioned by [1] and [2] have indeed disappeared.

In case of food, market access has indeed had an influence in Chácobo life. In the 1980’s cassava (Manihot esculenta, Euphorbiaceae) was clearly the most important food for Chácobo, and seven varieties were planted (Boom 1987). Maize (Zea mays), was planted on 18% of the land, and upland rice (Oryza sativa) was only planted on 7% of the land [7]. Nowadays rice has become the staple food of the Chácobo, leaving cassava and maize in a more secondary role, However, all original traditional maize and cassava varieties, as well as traditional banana varieties, are still grown. In our work we also found all edible species mentioned by Boom (1987) as planted in home– and forest–gardens, but the Chácobo had incorporated many additional species, e.g. lemon (Citrus sinensis, Rutaceae) in home gardens, and Psidium, Myrica sp. and Eugenia sp. in the forest gardens. One noteworthy exception was the palm Huanimá (Bactris gasipaes var. chichagui, Arecaceae), actively sown formerly in abandoned clearings to collect palm fruits [7]. In 2015 the palm was only found rarely around the villages, and was no longer planted.

The Chácobo keep using a large number of plants for medicinal purposes although missionaries of the Summer Institute of Linguistics tried to eradicate traditional medicinal plant use and traditional agricultural practices, because they regarded this as pagan [4]. Early anthropological and missionary accounts mentioned hardly any medicinal species [2, 4, 5], but this was clearly an interview artifact. Of the 360 plant species collected by Boom, 174 species were of medicinal value [7]. Bergeron recorded 399 useful plant species, of which 166 were classified as medicinal [8]. This compares favorably to the over 331 useful plant species elucidated in the current study. The Chácobo still favor the preparation of remedies by boiling the leaves, bark or fruits to cure diseases. While Boom did not find a true “healer” among the Chácobo [7], several Chácobo healers were identified in the present study. The knowledge of medicinal plants was particularly alive among older informants interviewed, but younger participants still retained much of such knowledge. The use of plant poisons, especially for fishing was mentioned as highly important by [7], and is still practiced today.

One of the most profound changes in Chácobo life seems to be a return to nomadic patterns, now mostly linked to commerce and income generation. The production of oil from the seeds of Brazil nuts (Berthollettia excels) was reported by Boom [7], but is little practiced nowadays – all nuts are now sold to large companies in Riberalta. The Brazil nut harvest takes place from January and March, and during that time now almost the entire Chácobo population migrates to the South of the territory where the largest concentration of Berthollettia is encountered. During the rest of the year Alto Ivón remains the main population center. However, many Chácobo have “second” homes in Tokyo, where most of the fields are located at present, or in Triangulo, closer to their main fishing sources, and conveniently located at the road to Riberalta.

Conclusions

In this paper we illustrate the complexity of perspectives on knowledge at different ages, and the persistence of knowledge over almost a century. We found that traditional knowledge was only partially affected by the processes of exposure to a market economy, and that different knowledge domains experienced different trends as a result of these changes. Overall knowledge was widely distributed, similar to [63]. However, we did not observe a directional knowledge loss, contrasting [64].

We stress the importance to not directly conclude processes of knowledge loss, cultural erosion or acculturation when comparing the knowledge of different age groups. These results should be treated with caution, because they cannot rule out the role of other variables affecting knowledge, including changes in the composition of other important factors that might be affected by the influence of access to a market economy. It is important to remember that learning, and accumulating experiences, require time. For this reason, the alternative explanation that the knowledge of older people tends to have accumulated over time, compared to the younger generation, should also be considered. It also needs to be taken into account that older generations might have different perceptions of their environment, because their points of reference are different from those of younger people. The ability to generate and apply knowledge in human populations enables actions and adjustments in response to current and future changes. Similarly, the ability to generate and apply knowledge, and not the knowledge itself, helps to increase the resilience of socio–ecological systems.

The analysis presented here clearly suggests that perceived knowledge “loss” might easily be an artifact of the researcher’s presence, of limited time, and of a very limited number of participants. Training local interviewers provides an excellent tool yield more reliable information on traditional knowledge and its potential loss in the future.

In compliance with the Nagoya Protocol, the original field notebooks, as well as the complete dataset, and a guide on useful plants of the Chácobo was repatriated to the Chácobo [65]. All members of the tribe have access to the compiled interview data for purposes of learning and education. The data collected are a valuable resource to the community as a tool to preserve their traditional knowledge, and will encourage the launch of research projects and community activities so the information does not become static. Species identified as being most important to the community can be targeted for conservation and restoration activities.

References

  1. Nordenskjöld E. Indianer und Weisse im Nordosten Boliviens. Stuttgart: Strecker und Schröder; 1922.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Haenke W. The Chácobo in Bolivia. Ethnos. 1958;23:100–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Prost G. Notas linguisticas de Bolivia II: Phonemas de la lengua Chácobo. La Paz, SIL: Instituto Linguistico de Verano; 1960.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Prost MD. Costumbres, habilidades y cuadro de vida entre los Chácobos. La Paz, SIL: Instituto Linguistico de Verano; 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Kelm H. Chácobo 1970. Tribus. 1972;21:129–246.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Córdoba LI. Misioneros-Patrones e indígenas-siringueros: El caucho entre los Chácobos del Beni (Siglo XX). Bol Americ. 2012;LXII(2–65):85–106.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Boom BM. Ethnobotany of the Chácobo Indians, Beni, Bolivia. Adv Econ Bot. 1987;4:1–68.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Bergeron S. El uso de las plantas por los Chácobos (Alto Ivón, Beni, Bolivia). La Paz: Institut Franjáis d'Etudes Andines (IFEA); 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Muñoz V, Sauvain M, Bourdy G, Callapa J, Bergeron S, Rojas I, Bravo JA, Balderrama L, Ortiz B, Gimenez A, Deharo E. A search for natural bioactive compounds in Bolivia through a multidisciplinary approach. Part I. Evaluation of the antimalarial activity of plants used by the Chácobo Indians. J Ethnopharmacol. 2000;69:127–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Huntington HP. Using traditional ecological knowledge in science: methods and applications. Ecol Appl. 2000;10(5):270–1274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Shackeroff JM, Campbell LM. 2007. Traditional ecological knowledge in conservation research: problems and prospects for their constructive engagement. Conserv Soc. 2007;5(3):343–60.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Begossi A. 1996. Use of ecological methods in Ethnobotany: diversity indices. Econ Bot. 1996;50(3):280–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Case RJ, Pauli G, Soejarto D, Ladio AH, Weigandt M. Cultural transmission of Ethnobotanical knowledge in a rural Community of Northwestern Patagonia. Argentina. Econ Bot. 2006;60:374–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Lozada M, Ladio AH, Weigandt M. Cultural transmission of Ethnobotanical knowledge in a rural Community of Northwestern Patagonia. Argentina Econ Bot. 2006;60:374–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Monteiro JM, Albuquerque UP. Freitas Lins–Neto EM, lima Araujo E, Cavalcanti Amorim E. Use patterns and knowledge of medicinal species among two rural communities in Brazil’s semi–arid northeastern region. J Ethnopharmacol. 2006;105:173–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Turner NJ, Turner K. Where our women used to get the food: cumulative effects and loss of Ethnobotanical knowledge and practice; case study from coastal British Columbia. Botany. 2008;86:103–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Benz BF, Cevallos E, Santana M, Rosales A, Graf S. Losing knowledge about plant use in the sierra de Mazatlan biosphere reserve. Mexico Econ Bot. 2000;54(2):183–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Stone GD. Agricultural deskilling and the spread of genetically modified cotton in Warangal. Curr Anth. 2007;48:67–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Gómez-Baggethun E, Reyes-García V. Reinterpreting change in traditional ecological knowledge. Hum Ecol. 2013;41(4):643–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Berkes F, Colding J, Folke C. 2000. Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. Ecol Appl. 2000;10(5):1251–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Colding J, Elmqvist T, Olsson P. Living with disturbance: building resilience in social–ecological systems. In: Berkes F, Colding J, Folke C, editors. Navigating social–ecological systems: building resilience for complexity and change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003. p. 163–73.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Berkes F. Community-based conservation in a globalized world. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104:15188–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Sternberg R, Nokes C, Geissler P, Prince R, Okatcha F, Bundy D, Grigorenko E. The relationship between academic and practical intelligence: a case study in Kenya. Intell. 2001;29:401–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Godoy R, Reyes-García V, Byron E, Leonard W, Vadez V. The effect of market economies on the wellbeing of indigenous peoples and on their use of renewable natural resources. Ann Rev Anth. 2005;34:121–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Reyes-García V, Vadez V, Byron E, Apaza L, Leonard WR, Perez E, Wilkie D. Market economy and the loss of folk knowledge of plant uses: estimates from the Tsimane’ of the Bolivian Amazon. Curr Anth. 2005;46(4):651–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Hanazaki N, Tamashiro JY, Leitão-Filho HF, Begossi A. Diversity of plant uses in two Caiçara communities form the Atlantic forest coast. Brazil Biodiv Cons. 2000;9:597–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Godoy R, Reyes-García V, Broesch J, Fitzpatrick IC, Giovarmini P, MRM R, Jha N, Huanca T, Leonard WR, TW MD, Tanner S, TAPS Bolivia Study Team. Long-term (secular) change of ethnobotanical knowledge of useful plants separating cohort and age effects. J Anth Res. 2009;65(1):51–67.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Reyes-García V, Guèze M, Luz AC, Paneque-Gálvez J, Macía MJ, Orta-Martinez M, Pino J, Rubio-Campillo X. Evidence of traditional knowledge loss among a contemporary indigenous society. Evol Hum Behav. 2013;34(4):249–57.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Reyes-García V, Luz AC, Gueze M, Paneque-Gálvez J, Macia MJ, Orta-Martínez M, Pino J. TAPS Bolivian study team. Secular trends on traditional ecological knowledge: an analysis of different domains of knowledge among Tsimane’ men. Learn Indiv Diff. 2013;27:206–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Mackinson S, Nottestad L. Combining local and scientific knowledge. Rev Fish Bio Fisheries. 1998;8(4):481–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Luoga EJ, Witkowski ETF, Balkwill K. Differential utilization and ethnobotany of trees in Kitulanghalo forest reserve and surrounding communal lands, eastern Tanzania. Econ Bot. 2000;54(3):328–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Byg A. Humans and plants of the rain forest: factors affecting local knowledge and use of plants. University of Aarhus: Doctoral thesis; Department of Systematic Botany; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Byg A, Balslev H. Palms in indigenous and settler communities in southeastern Ecuador: farmers’ perceptions and cultivation practices. Agrofor Syst. 2006;67:147–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Paniagua–Zambrana NY, Byg A, Svenning JC, Moraes M, Grandez C, Balslev H. Diversity of palm uses in the western Amazon. Biodivers Conserv. 2007;16:2771–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Byg A, Balslev H. Diversity and use of palms in Zahamena, eastern Madagascar. Biodivers Conserv. 2001;10:951–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Byg A, Balslev H. Factors affecting local knowledge of palms in Nangaritza Valley in South–Eastern Ecuador. J Ethnobiol. 2004;24(2):255–78.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Takasaki Y, Barham BL, Coomes OT. Amazonian peasants, rain forest use and income generation: the role of wealth and geographical factors. Soc Nat Res. 2001;14:291–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Byg A, Vormisto J, Balslev H. Influence of diversity and road access on palm extraction at landscape scale in SE Ecuador. Biodivers Conserv. 2007;16:631–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Vandebroek I. The dual Intracultural and intercultural relationship between medicinal plant knowledge and consensus. Econ Bot. 2010;64(4):303–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Alburquerque UP, Muniz de Medeiros P. Systematic reviews and meta–analysis applied to Ethnobiological research. Ethnobiol Cons. 2012;1:6.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Moerman DE, Pemberton RW, Kiefer D. A comparative analysis of five medicinal floras. J Ethnobiol. 1999;19(1):49–67.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Molares S, Ladio A. Ethnobotanical review of the Mapuche medicinal flora: use patterns on a regional scale. J Ethnopharmacol. 2009;122(2):251–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Saslis-Lagoudakis CH, Williamson EM, Savolainen V, Hawkins JA. Cross-cultural comparison of three medicinal floras and implications for bioprospecting strategies. J Ethnopharmacol. 2011;135(2):476–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Zent S. Acculturation and Ethnobotanical knowledge loss among the Piaroa of Venezuela: demonstration of a quantitative method for the empirical study of TEK change. In: Maffi L, editor. On biocultural diversity: linking language, knowledge, and the environment. Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution Press; 2001. p. 190–211.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Bussmann RW, Paniagua Zambrana NY. Traditional knowledge in a changing world – new insights from the Chácobo in Bolivia. In: Ponman B, Bussmann RW, editors. Medicinal plants and the legacy of Richard E. Schultes. Proc. botany 2011. Trujillo: Graficart; 2012. p. 23–34.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Paniagua Zambrana N, Bussmann RW, Blacutt E, Macia MJ, editors. Los Chácobo y las Palmeras. Graficart; 2011.

  47. Bussmann RW, Paniagua Zambrana NY. La etnobotanica de los Chácobo: Traduccion de boom B:the Ethnobotany of the Chácobo Indians, Beni, Bolivia. Trujillo: Graficart; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Paniagua Zambrana NY, Bussmann RW, Tellez C, Vega C. Los Chácobo y su historia en el siglo XX. St. Louis: William L. Brown Center, MBG; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Paniagua Zambrana NY, Macía MJ, Cámara-Leret R. Toma de datos etnobotánicos de palmeras y variables socioeconómicas en comunidades rurales - Ethnobotanical data gathering of palms and socio–economic variables in rural communities. Ecol Bol. 2010;45(3):44–68.

    Google Scholar 

  50. International Society of Ethnobiology. International Society of Ethnobiology Code of Ethics (with 2008 additions). Society of Ethnobiology; 2008. http://ethnobiology.net/code–of–ethics/. Accessed 7 July 2017.

  51. Oksanen J, Blanchet FG, Kindt R, Legendre P, Minchin PR, O'Hara, RB, Simpson GL, Solymos P, Henry M, Stevens H, Wagner H. vegan: Community Ecology Package. 2016. https://CRAN.R–project.org/package=vegan.

  52. Dufrene M, Legendre P. Species assemblages and indicator species: the need for a flexible asymmetrical approach. Ecol Monogr. 1997;67(3):345–66.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Roberts DW. labdsv: Ordination and Multivariate Analysis for Ecology. 2016. http://ecology.msu.montana.edu/labdsv/R.

  54. Phillips O, Gentry AH. The useful plants of Tambopata, Peru: II. Additional hypothesis testing in quantitative ethnobotany. Econ Bot. 1993;47(1):33–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Zent S. The quandary of conserving ethnoecological knowledge – a Piaroa example. In: Gragson TL, Blount BG, editors. Ethnoecology - knowledge, resources, and rights. Athens: University of Georgia Press; 1999. p. 90–124.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Godoy R, Brokaw N, Wilkie D, Coloón D, Palermo A, Lye S, Wei S. On trade and cognition: markets and the loss of folk knowledge among the Tawahka Indians. J Anth Res. 1998;54:219–33.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Zarger R, Stepp JR. Persistence of botanical knowledge among Tzeltal Maya children. Curr Anth. 2004;45:413–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Atran S, Medin D, Ross N, Lynch E, Vapnarsky V, Ucan Ek E, Coley J, Timura C, Baran M. Folkecology, cultural epidemiology and the spirit of the commons. Curr Anth. 2002;43:421–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Lawrence A, Philipps OL, Reategui A, López M, Rose S, Wood D. Local values for harvested forest plants in Madre de Dios, Peru: towards a more contextualized interpretation of quantitative Ethnobotanical data. Biodivers Conserv. 2005;14:45–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Harrison KD. When languages die: the extinction of the World's languages and the erosion of human knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  61. Zarger RK. Acquisition and transmission of subsistence knowledge by Q’eqchi’ Maya in Belize. In: Stepp JR, Wyndham FS, Zarger RK, editors. Etnobiology and biocultural diversity. Athens: International Society of Ethnobiology; 2002. p. 592-603.

  62. Guest G. Market integration and the distribution of ecological knowledge within an Ecuadorian fishing community. J Ecol Anth. 2002;6:38–49.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Reyes-García V, Godoy R, Vadez V, Apaza L, Byron E, Huanca T, Leonard WR, Pérez E, Wilkie D. Ethnobotanical knowledge shared widely among Tsimane’ Amerindians. Bolivia Sci. 2003;29:1707.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Reyes-García V, Gueze M, Luz AC, Paneque-Galvez J, Macía MJ, Orta-Martínez M, Pino J, Rubio-Campillo X. Evidence of traditional knowledge loss among a contemporary indigenous society. Evol Hum Behav. 2013;34(4):249–57. Doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Paniagua-Zambrana NY, Bussmann RW. La Etnobotánica de los Chácobo en el Siglo XXI. St. Louis: William L. Brown Center; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We greatly thank Ravi Ortiz, President of the Central Indígena de la Región Amazónica de Bolivia (CIRABO), and Maro Ortiz, Capitan General of the TCO Chácobo, as well as all our Chácobo friends and counterparts, and the whole Chácobo population for all their friendship and support. We thank the National Geographic Society (Grant #9244–13) for support of the fieldwork.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Geographic Society (Grant 9244–13) and endowment funds of the William L. Brown Center at Missouri Botanical Garden, for which we are grateful.

Availability of data and materials

The raw data contain the names of all participants, and cannot be shared publicly. Data without participant data can be obtained upon request after an access and benefit sharing agreement agreement with CIRABO.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

NYPZ and RBU designed the study; NYPZ, RBU, ALHM, GOS, MOV, DOA, JSM, MSM, SC, BCM, GCM and ES conducted the fieldwork; ALMH curated and identified the collections and entered the original data; NYPZ and RBU analyzed the data and NYPZ, RBU and NPZ wrote the manuscript; REH conducted the statistical analysis; all authors read, corrected and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Narel Y. Paniagua Zambrana.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Before conducting interviews, both the permission of CIRABO, and individual prior informed consent was obtained from all participants. No further ethics approval was required. All work conducted was carried out under the stipulations of the Nagoya Protocol on Access to Genetic Resources and the Fair and Equitable Sharing of Benefits Arising from their Utilization to the Convention on Biological Diversity. The right to use and authorship of any traditional knowledge of all participants is maintained, and any use of this information, other than for scientific publication, does require additional prior consent of the traditional owners, as well as a consensus on access to benefits resulting from subsequent use.

Consent for publication

This manuscript does not contain any individual person’s data and further consent for publication is not required.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Paniagua Zambrana, N.Y., Bussmann, R.W., Hart, R.E. et al. Traditional knowledge hiding in plain sight – twenty-first century ethnobotany of the Chácobo in Beni, Bolivia. J Ethnobiology Ethnomedicine 13, 57 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-017-0179-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-017-0179-2

Keywords