Skip to main content

Short-term temporal analysis and children's knowledge of the composition of important medicinal plants: the structural core hypothesis

Abstract

Background

Measures of the importance of medicinal plants have long been used in ethnobotany and ethnobiology to understand the influence of social-ecological system factors in the formation of individuals’ differential knowledge and use. However, there is still a gap in empirical studies that seek to understand the temporal aspects of this process.

Methods

To overcome this issue, we used the concept of the structural core of medicinal plants, a theoretical-evolutionary model, which argues that the importance of medicinal plant resources is related to the increase in individual and population fitness. It represents the set of the most effective and available resources that would treat the most common diseases in an environment. This composition of knowledge would be conservative over space and time. To test these questions, we hypothesized that the composition of the structural core remains constant during temporal changes in a social-ecological context, and that the composition of the infantile structural core (new generation) is similar to that of the adults (older generation). For 2 years, we tracked the structure of important medicinal plants among the same 49 residents of a community located in Vale do Catimbau in Pernambuco, Brazil. We also compared the importance of the medicinal plants among two different generations, children/adolescents and adults, in the same space/time context.

Results and Conclusion

Our results refuted both hypotheses. Regarding the composition of important medicinal plants through temporal variations and for children's learning, our results were not predicted by the model. This suggests that the structural core should not be regarded as a conservative phenomenon, but rather a congenital, dynamic, and plastic occurrence that has adapted to configure itself as a short-term population response to the treatment of local diseases.

Introduction

The knowledge and local use of medicinal plants are characteristics of medical systems, which are systematically analyzed in ethnobotany and ethnobiology. Many studies consider this cultural domain fundamental to allowing subsistence societies to survive the different challenges present in social-ecological systems [1,2,3,4]. However, some studies on the structure of this knowledge have shown that human populations have a unique tendency to practice and recognize the importance of only a few sets of medicinal plants, despite the vast repertoire of culture that can be accumulate and pass through generations on the medicinal properties of local ecological resources [2, 5,6,7]. In this sense, our study starts with the objective of investigating whether there are intrinsic properties of the systems that influence the construction and conservation of this knowledge structure based on importance. We also seek to track the composition of the knowledge over time.

The importance of plant resources has been studied and measured in different ways in ethnobiology and ethnobotany. For example, Ferreira Júnior et al. [8] used the cultural salience index of species [9, 10] to identify the most preferred species for the treatment of inflammation. Bonifácio et al. [11] defined the cultural importance of certain plant species within the systems to elaborate and discuss priority conservation strategies using the use value index [12, 13]. Medeiros et al. [14] defined the most important knowledge using the relative importance index [12, 15] to discuss the differential use of medicinal plants according to their taste and smell. Characterizing the importance of medicinal plants in ethnobotanical research can also be defined via measures of cultural significance [6, 16, 17].

These varied ways of measuring the “cultural importance” of a particular medicinal resource show us how difficult it is to develop a definitive measure of the cultural and practical significance of a particularly renown taxon [18]. The cultural significance of certain elements in the system is a natural process of the structure of the populational knowledge of societies [19,20,21]. Therefore, all these analyses and scientific metrics only seek to significantly delimit those that researchers can define as relevant, or not, to formulate their investigations and work questions.

There is still a lack of studies in ethnobotany that seek to investigate the evolution of important knowledge over time, with some exceptions [22, 23]. These studies may take the form of tracking the components of this knowledge in punctual collections through space/time [such as through free lists or questionnaires] or investigating how many new generations [children and adolescents] “absorb” the knowledge of plant species from this structure. The measurement and characterization of important medicinal plants are, in a way, tied to the explanatory variables that make up the current social-ecological context. There may be important types of knowledge that are historically significant for the population studied. However, to our knowledge, virtually no study has conducted successive interviews to try to analyze the variation, or lack thereof, in the knowledge considered important by local populations. How should the body of the knowledge of important medicinal plants behave across space and time? Our study seeks to advance this critical question to enrich ethnobotanical theory and practice, using the concept of the structural core of medicinal plants as a theoretical-evolutionary model of the knowledge in medical systems [2].

The concept of the structural core, developed by Ferreira Júnior and Albuquerque [2], is based on the hypothesis that medicinal plants with high popularity [core] have adaptive traits that address the therapeutic needs in a medical system. Important medicinal knowledge is characterized as a cultural “basic kit” for dealing with diseases in the environment. Based on the literature, the authors argue that these plants become important because they have adaptive traits, represented by the plant’s efficacy in the treatment of diseases, availability in the environment, and treatment of common diseases, thereby providing the best available chance to speed up the cure of an illness that incapacitates the people in local communities [2]. This would allow the efficient “real-time” maintenance of local disease treatment processes. Thus, with the passage of space and time, in a non-stochastic ecological way the knowledge of these medicinal plants tends to accumulate in knowledge systems [24, 25]. Therefore, the selection of important medicinal plant species would be aimed at long-term conservation.

This hypothesis is supported by the adaptive memory model [26], in which individuals are genetically “tuned” to more efficiently memorize a behavior evaluated to be adaptive to the context [27,28,29]. In addition to this is the fact that local populations share the recognition of “prototypical” medicinal plants, which are those that are a reference for the treatment of common diseases, through mnemonic clues such as taste and smell [5, 30]. In this sense, important medicinal practices that involve the knowledge and use of medicinal plants that are perceived as more effective or available, and thus provide a greater advantage of fitness, would automatically or preparedly stimulate the synaptic connections selected over time to solve medical problems.

These two cognitive and cultural-referential structures work together to modulate the structural core in an increasingly “conservative” disposition across space and time, preserving intergenerational knowledge of the local medicinal plants that are most important in the treatment of diseases.

Other evolutionary theories, such as the theory of cumulative cultural evolution and natural pedagogy, can be used as a guide in explaining the adaptive importance of medicinal plants. The theory of cumulative cultural evolution [31] refers to the process through which cultural traditions are gradually modified and improved over time [32]. This process allows societies to “accumulate” solutions to problems faced by individuals that could not have been invented by a single person [33]. It also enables individuals to reach responses in a shared way, thereby distributing the efforts, risks, and time of the trial-and-error processes [32]. For example, let us consider the practice of using tea as a remedy for stomach aches. Boiling the water, selecting the medicinal plant parts, and determining the time required to extract the therapeutic properties via infusion are problem-solving behaviors that are not likely to be practiced by someone new to the idea of treating stomach pain-related diseases. This complex package of information about medical systems can accumulate in the systems and be passed on in blocks to new cultural learners. According to the theory of natural pedagogy, during childhood, children are the most favored in social learning. This is because, in addition to having cognitive mechanisms that allow them to learn social information quickly and effectively, adults tend to facilitate the learning environmental information by directly teaching the most adaptive ones [34, 35].

Thus, based on the assumptions proposed above, to investigate the temporal evolution of the important medicinal plants remembered by people, we hypothesize that (H1) the composition of the structural core remains constant during the temporal variation in a social-ecological context. We expect that (P1), concerning satellite plants [plants that are not part of the structural core], the composition of medicinal plant species from the structural core at Period 1 will be similar to the composition of species of medicinal plants from the structural core at Period 2. In addition, to analyze how much the new generations remember the medicinal plants considered to be important by the older generation, we hypothesized that (H2) the composition of the infantile structural core [new generation] is similar to that of the adults [older generation]. We expect that (P2), concerning the satellite plants, the composition of medicinal plant species in the infantile structural core is similar to that of the adult structural core.

Materials and methods

Characterization of the area and study sites

The study was performed within the National Park of Vale do Catimbau (PARNA do Catimbau; 8°32′54.2″ S 37°14′49.6″ W). This park is an Integral Protection Conservation Unit in the state of Pernambuco, in the northeast region of Brazil, which had its limits federalized on December 13, 2002. Consequently, the ecological resources of its interior are protected by law [36]. This unit covers an area of 64,294 ha and is located within the municipalities of Ibimirim, Tupanatinga, and Buíque (Fig. 1). The climate is classified as tropical semiarid with a Caatinga (Brazilian savannah) ecosystem, an annual average temperature of approximately 23 °C, and precipitation between 486 and 975 mm, with rainfall concentrated between March and July [37]. The nearest village and entrance to PARNA do Catimbau is Vila do Catimbau, which has approximately 2240 inhabitants [38]. There, small markets, building material stores, a health center, and an elementary school can be found. However, access to Vila do Catimbau is difficult because of the presence of precarious mud roads. Prolonged drought and lack of rain are hallmarks of this region.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Location map of the territorial limits of PARNA do Catimbau, Vila do Catimbau, and the communities studied from 2017 to 2019

There are at least 17 small communities distributed across PARNA do Catimbau [37]. All communities operate in subsistence mode as its residents collect resources from vegetation to use as natural remedies, firewood, and for building houses and fences. Some residents own small bean and corn plantations. Residents also raise grazing animals, such as goats and cattle, and backyard animals, such as chickens, for food or sale. Water is a scarce resource that keeps people within a permanent rationing system. There are no health facilities close to the communities inside the park; as a result, individuals with medical emergencies of any kind must be carried out in some way, either to the Vila do Catimbau Health Care or to the large urban centers of the nearby municipalities. Likewise, there are no schools of any kind inside the park; however, the children receive the assistance of the government for transportation to carry out their studies in Vila do Catimbau. In addition, some families within PARNA do Catimbau receive food, as well as educational, social, and citizen assistance from the non-governmental organization Amigos do Bem [www.amigosdobem.org], whose edifice is located at the entrance to Vila do Catimbau. This organization transfers goods and services that are deemed national and international financial aid for these residents.

Before making contact with any residents of the region, we met with the head of the Vila do Catimbau Family Health Post to explain our research objectives. We also asked for general information about their way of life, and the number of residents and residences located within the PARNA do Catimbau.

Ethical and legal aspects

We spent several weeks building rapport within the local communities. All interviewees (> 18 years old) who agreed to participate in this study were invited to sign the Free and Informed Consent form. Concerning the interview with the children (aged 6 to 14 years), all responsible adults (> 18 years) were invited to sign a second Free and Informed Consent form authorizing the participation of the children. Additionally, for the children who agreed to participate, we invited them to sign (or place their fingerprint on) another specific consent form in keeping with the current legislation of the Ministry of Health (Resolution 466/12 of the National Health Council). This legally protects them by keeping their identities and shared information confidential.

This study was submitted and approved by the Official State Body of the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Pernambuco (CAAE:89887817.6.0000.5207).

Ethnobotanical data collection

Data collection for this research was carried out in two periods; the first was conducted between January and November 2017, and the second was between January and April 2019. The initial contact was done with a team of researchers associated with the National Institute of Science and Technology in Ethnobiology, Bioprospection, and Nature Conservation (https://www.inctethnobio.com/) and the Laboratory of Ecology and Evolution of Socioecological Systems (https://www.evoethnobio.com). On this event, several weeks were dedicated to building the local people's trust in our research group.

For all participants interviewed (adults or children), we used the free-list method to record local the knowledge of medicinal plants [9, 12]. This method was chosen because, in addition to being semantically efficient in acquiring abundant amounts of information about local knowledge, it is simple and objective in its application, which makes it possible to maximize the number of interviews. The interview consisted of asking a single question, “Which medicinal plants do you know?” without additional stimuli. The responses allowed the order of recall of the medicinal plants per person to be recorded. As much as possible, conducting the interviews in an individualized manner was prioritized to prevent other people from stimulating crossed memories.

In total, there were five rural communities located inside the park where the interviews were conducted: Igrejinha, Muquém, Dor de Dente, Túnel, and Açude Velho (Fig. 1). All of them have similar forms of housing, and the residents had similar lifestyles among themselves and other communities located within the park. According to a survey by our group of researchers, Igrejinha has approximately 112 residents and 51 homes, Muquém has 38 residents and 21 homes, Dor de Dente has 13 residents and 7 homes, Túnel has 6 residents and 2 homes, and Açude Velho has 17 residents and 10 homes. We interviewed 49 residents from these five communities.

To test the first research hypothesis, we interviewed the same 49 participants (> 18 years old) in periods 1 and 2. Specifically, 33 participants were in Igrejinha, eight in Muquém, two in Dor de Dente, three in Túnel, and three in Açude Velho. To test the second research hypothesis, we focused on the community of Muquém and interviewed 25 adult participants [> 18 years old] and 18 child participants [aged between 6 and 14 years] in Period 2. It is important to note that we decided to consider children in this age group because ethnobotanical studies suggest that it is around this age that a person in a subsistence context in social-ecological systems begins to become “culturally apt” to develop and use the knowledge and practices of their society [17, 39].

In summary, to test the first hypothesis, we carried out free lists of medicinal plants with the same 49 participants at an interval of approximately 2 years (periods 1 and 2). To test the second hypothesis, we carried out free lists of medicinal plants for 25 adults and 18 children in the same period and community.

Data analysis

All our predictions initially depended on the definition of which plants we will consider to be part of the structural core, in other words, which plants make up the set of important medicinal plants, and which plants are not regarded as part of it. As noted in the introduction, we will refer to non-structural core plants as “satellite plants.” Because Ferreira Júnior and Albuquerque [2] suggested that core plants have intrinsic adaptive characteristics that make them more “popular” within a local medical system, we decided to measure this popularity through the cultural salience index [9, 12], an analysis that takes into account both the frequency and the recall order of all the information recorded in the free lists. In this analysis, the more frequent and first remembered an item, the more salient [or important for the culture in question] it is.

However, this method of analysis results in a list of items on a decreasing numerical continuum, and any further attempt to divide this list into “structural core plants” and “satellite plants” would be subjective and could produce a biased interpretation of the results. To solve this impasse of objectivity, we used the statistical output defined by Chaves et al. [40], called the “salience threshold.”

Chaves et al. [40] developed a method of analysis that compares the salience list obtained in the field with 1000 salient lists generated in a statistical environment simulating a null scenario. For the null scenario to have the same characteristics as the real scenario in the field, these salience lists are based on hypothetical free lists that are randomly generated, according to the same number of participants, the same number of items cited by the population, and the same average citations per participant [40]. Using this statistic, salience values obtained from the field should be significantly different from those expected by the salience lists generated by the null scenario, with items that have higher or lower values than those expected by chance. In this sense, the “salience threshold” defines which items become more prominent when comparing the observed values of salience with the expected values against the null model, with a decision alpha of 5% [40]. This method of analysis allowed us to characterize the knowledge structure in structural core plants and satellite plants of the 49 people in periods 1 and 2, and of the 25 adults and 18 children from Muquém in Period 2.

After statistically defining the “salience threshold,” to directly test hypotheses 1 and 2, we used a permutational multivariate analysis of variance (PERMANOVA) [41]. The data matrices were constructed in Excel as follows: for Hypothesis 1, the rows presented each of the 49 participants from Period 1; this was followed by the same 49 participants, only now related to Period 2. The columns were composed of all the medicinal plants contained in the contextual structural core of periods 1 and 2. If the participant remembered any medicinal plant contained in the columns, we marked “1”; otherwise we marked “0.” The same procedure for the core plants was also performed for the construction of the related matrices for Hypothesis 2; however, instead of relating the participants' lines by period, we related them by “adults” and “children” [see Additional files 1, 2: data].

The Jaccard index was used to calculate the dissimilarity index using presence/absence data matrices. All dissimilarity indices were calculated using the vegdist function of the vegan 2.5–7 package in the R development environment [42]. The homogeneity of the multivariate dispersions was calculated to analyze the variation in species composition using the betadisper function of the vegan package [43]. For a visual analysis of the dissimilarity, we plotted the principal coordinates (PCoA) using the base plot function of R.

Results

In the first period, the 49 participants recalled 81 medicinal plants. Using the “salient threshold,” we characterized the 15 most salient plants as the structural core, and the remaining 66 were regarded as satellites. In the second period, 2 years later, the same participants recalled 77 medicinal plants, 11 were characterized as the structural core and 66 as satellites (Table 1). The Jaccard dissimilarity index indicated that the composition of medicinal plants present in the structural core in Period 1 is significantly different from that present in the structural core of Period 2 (p < 0.001). These results refute our first hypothesis.

Table 1 Structure of knowledge of important medicinal plants from the 49 residents of Vale do Catimbau, 33 in Igrejinha, 8 in Muquém, 2 in Dor de Dente, 3 in Túnel, and 3 in Açude Velho [NE Brazil], in the years 2017 and 2019, characterized by a structural core and satellite plans after calculating the “salient threshold”

Regarding the second hypothesis, the 25 adults from Muquém recalled 53 medicinal plants, and, after the “salient threshold,” 9 were categorized as structural core plants and 44 were categorized as satellite plants. The 18 children recalled 22 medicinal plants, two of which were characterized as structural core plants and 20 as satellite plants (Table 2). The Jaccard dissimilarity index showed that the composition of medicinal plants present in the structural core of the adults was significantly different from that of the children (p < 0.001). Thus, these results also refute the second hypothesis. A summary of all the statistics reported above is presented in Table 3 for Hypothesis 1 and in Table 4 for Hypothesis 2. The visual analysis of the dissimilarity of both tests, resulting from plots of the main coordinates of our data, is presented in Figs. 2 and 3.

Table 2 Structure of knowledge about important medicinal plants of the 25 adults and 18 children living in the community of Muquém in Vale do Catimbau (NE Brazil), characterized by structural core and satellite plants after calculating the “salient threshold.” This information was taken in the same space/time context in 2019
Table 3 Permutational multivariate analysis of variance (PERMANOVA) results from testing the similarity of the composition of medicinal plant species that were characterized as structural core plants by the same 49 residents of Vale do Catimbau in the two interview periods of 2017 and 2019
Table 4 Permutational multivariate analysis of variance (PERMANOVA) results from testing the similarity of the composition of medicinal plant species, characterized by the 25 adults and 18 children living in the community of Muquém in Vale do Catimbau (NE Brazil). These results were measured in the same space/time context in 2019
Fig. 2
figure 2

Visual analysis of the dissimilarity of the data from Hypothesis 1, resulting from plots of the main coordinates from Period 1 (t1) in 2017 and from Period 2 (t2) in 2019

Fig. 3
figure 3

Visual analysis of the dissimilarity of the data from Hypothesis 2, resulting from plots of the main coordinates from adults and children in Period 2 in 2019

Discussion

As seen above, our results indicate the complete opposite of the evolutionary model of medical systems based on the concept of a structural core that was predicted by Ferreira Júnior and Albuquerque [2]. The composition of important medicinal plants is not supported by the assumptions of specific adaptive long-term factors. Other more dynamic factors must also be considered. Our results allow us to affirm that the composition of plants from the structural core is not in a constant adaptive search for crystallization in the memory and recall of individuals in free lists. Moreover, at the population level, it has a very “volatile” lifespan from a temporal point of view. Nevertheless, we can also say that the importance of medicinal plants can be completely dependent on the current and historical temporal contexts.

Short-term temporal adjustment?

According to the structural core model [2], the composition of medicinal plants considered most important by people is the result of medical system processes that involve efficacy, availability, the type of disease treatment, and social learning about plant resources. Therefore, while these factors are very important for the formation of the structural core, the change in the composition of plant species over time may be influenced by other factors of the system.

Several studies in the literature show how these processes can modulate behaviors that involve the knowledge and use of the plants considered to be important. Casagrande [30], for example, showed that factors such as availability and frequency of use, as well as the social and family roles that a person plays in society, directly influence their therapeutic choices. In a meta-analysis on the use value of useful plants, Gonçalves et al. [44] showed that factors, such as the local availability of plant resources, influence the differential use of firewood. This is because people may deliberately choose to use the plants available within their environment over others. Despite some opposing evidence [45], knowledge about medicinal plants of greater use value seems to be unaffected by phytosociological vegetation metrics [44]. Santos et al. [46] showed that plants considered important for different uses in the treatment of common diseases are strongly related to people's perception of them as effective for the treatment or cure [6, 47, 48].

The structural core model predicted that these important medicinal plants assemble all the necessary characteristics to act as protagonists in the treatment of diseases in medical systems, because they are perceived as most effective and/or available. It was expected that this set of information would accumulate over the long term, thereby creating a referential “basic kit” for the population over time.

However, as evidenced by our results, the structural core hypothesis was refuted. Such a “basic kit” did not exist, at least in the sense of targeting the specialization of medical systems through long-term cultural evolution. Perhaps we were not able to detect any pattern due to our time frame of 2 years of analysis. Although the structural core hypothesis was refuted, we believe that the only way to answer this question would be to invest in future long-term longitudinal studies to verify patterns in the composition of plant species considered important for the treatment of diseases over time.

Our findings indicate that the composition of the structural core is significantly more dynamic than that predicted by the model. It is always good to remember that most ethnobotanical studies, and socio-cultural and ecological data collection, are carried out at a specific point in space and time. Therefore, the 2015 model did not consider that the formation of the composition of important medicinal plants must have processes aimed at solving short-term spatial/temporal context challenges in social-ecological systems, in addition to those for long-term ones. Different disease episodes could greatly affect an individual’s patterns of remembering and using medicinal plants in daily life [49,50,51]. Our results indicate that there must be zones in the knowledge structure of medicinal plants that have very flexible compositions in the face of temporal variations.

The first question we can ask about this result is, “What are the factors that modulate this non-conservative temporal plasticity?” The second is whether a dynamic and plastic structural core adds an adaptive advantage to social-ecological systems. Furthermore, “Is the composition of important medicinal plants a contingent context-dependent phenomenon in medical systems?” Answering these questions will not be easy because there are many dimensions of systems to consider (such as socio-environmental, ecological, socio-cultural, and political) [52].

We believe that the explanation of our results is related to the hypothesis that important medicinal plants, based on their structural core, are knowledge structures created as a result of solving everyday short-term medical challenges. Therefore, the importance of medicinal plants must be a contingency phenomenon. The only way to test this hypothesis would be to carry out long-term studies with replicated experimental designs aimed at investigating the factors behind the formation of important medicinal plants both in context and over time. Although our study initially discarded the idea of the “basic kit,” it is expected that clear patterns may be found with 5 or 6 years of annual follow-ups. Only then will we be able to confirm or deny questions such as if there are conserved zones of the structural core with significant variations in the composition of important plants. The natural dynamics of ecological availability and the epidemiological context may be guiding the species composition of important plants.

Thus, to understand whether ecological availability is somehow related to the plasticity of the structural core over time, in periodic collections we must investigate the location and quantity of each of the important medicinal plants. Will they be the ones that are most commonly found in people's medicinal backyards? Will they be the most abundant ones in adjacent or exploited forests? Are they fully available to the community? Or are they anywhere near the studied communities? The fundamental method would be to simultaneously collect the same response variables (e.g., structural core and local preference) and explanatory variables (e.g., ecological availability) to investigate the patterns of the context, and also collect the minimum measurements of the same factors in the same way annually in long-term ethnobotanical research.

Epidemiological context can also be one of the main factors of medical systems in the modulation of a short-term adjusted and plastic structural core. In the original model of the structural core [2], it is argued that the composition of the most popular medicinal plants reflects the long-term evolutionary process in the treatment of common local diseases. Therefore, this “adjustment” of the composition of plants would be directed toward diseases that are always present in social-ecological systems. Therefore, if this composition adjustment is long term, 2 years of space/time variation should essentially capture the same set of recalled medicinal plants. However, our data suggest the opposite. We believe that what explains the plastic variation in the structural core, as captured by our measurements and relative to the local epidemiological context of the population at the time of the interviews, is how common the disease in question is [is it historical, seasonal, or newly established] and how frequently medicinal plants are used for their treatment.

Nevertheless, we must consider that, in addition to frequency, diseases have a specific severity level and latency period associated with them [3, 53,54,55]. Despite the severity of diseases inducing a low total richness of the knowledge of medicinal plant species for their treatment, because people tend to experience fewer new cure alternatives for illnesses with a high risk of death, these “idiosyncratic” medicinal plants may be related to the specific therapeutic resilience of the interviewed individual [54]. Furthermore, previous experience with these types of diseases can affect the individual’s recall rate of important medicinal plants.

Another variable of medical systems associated with the epidemiological context, which is the frequency of the use of plant resources, may also explain the plasticity of our results. Whether the diseases are common or uncommon, it is expected that the frequency of use of plant resources will indirectly influence the composition of the structural core. Casagrande [30], for example, directly verified the influence of the frequency of medicinal plant use on the formation of the composition of important medicinal plants [measured by salience]. In this way, Casagrande empirically showed that the more the plant is used, the more remembered the plant becomes. Priority is given to remembering those plant resources that are re-experienced daily or are significantly important to the person interviewed [49,50,51]. Reyes-Garcia et al. [51], however, showed that we can have plants rarely reported in interviews that are frequently used, and plants that are often reported in interviews and not used much. Our point here is that the frequency of use of medicinal plants in episodes of diseases may be modifying the rate of medicinal plant prioritization in a way that is not expected by the theoretical models of the structural core.

Thus, it is expected that over a 2-year period, individuals and populations may have several episodes of different diseases, which may be common or severe and historical or emerging. Moreover, the frequency of use of medicinal plants in each type of treatment varies. Accumulatively, this is what we believe may be influencing the plasticity of the structural core composition over time. Future studies that seek to better understand the short-term evolutionary dynamics of the composition of important plants, from the perspective of the frequency and severity of diseases, and the frequency of plant species use, could investigate measuring these epidemiological variables through methods such as residential therapeutic calendars, a survey of health posts, and individual perception [56].

Medicinal plants considered important to adults are not the same as those considered important to children and adolescents

Another explanatory hypothesis of the functioning of the structural core model proposed by Ferreira Júnior and Albuquerque [2] is that adults are the fundamental learning pathways for the consolidation of a child’s knowledge about important things in the environment. As the structural core would be the population reference for adaptive information to deal with diseases that circulate in social-ecological systems, it was expected that important medicinal plants among adults would also be the most important among the younger generations of the community. However, our similarity data refuted this hypothesis. The first question we can ask regarding this is why medicinal plants considered important by adults do not seem to exert a referential influence on those seen as important for children. It is important to point out that it is more plausible to consider that children seem to share knowledge that is different from that of adults [57, 58]. In addition, it is noteworthy to mention that we do not have the effect of the influence of the factors mentioned above for the first hypothesis [temporal variation], because all the interviewees, children and adults, were inserted in the same social-ecological space/time contexts.

We must ask ourselves, “How do children learn and reason about medical and medicinal plant knowledge through their development in social-ecological systems?” The formation of the memory of children has already been widely debated in the recent literature on cognitive psychology [59, 60]. Nevertheless, for many decades, the study of children's cognition has always been neglected in scientific ethnobotany/ethnobiology discussions, based on the assumption that children are mere assistants in the process of acquiring information from the environment. However, recent studies have shown that the knowledge of children can be specific to their day-to-day activities and, when viewed from a group perspective, may even have a quality of their own as a type of “children's culture” [57, 58]. Empirical research also suggests that the prevalence of vertical transmission can be overestimated in self-reports about the learning model [57]. Moreover, because children learn a lot from other children, this indicates the importance of horizontal transmission in the formation of knowledge and child practice [61].

Several ethnobotany studies have discussed the role of parents and guardians in the cultural learning of children and adolescents. These studies have shown the importance of this vertical information transmission route for the formation of knowledge among young people [39, 57, 62]. They have also shown that medicinal plants that are either easier to use or easier to find close to homes are generally those that are incorporated into the knowledge of children, whereas medicinal plants from the forest are better known by adults [63]. It is worth noting that the knowledge of children is closely associated with specific aspects of social-ecological systems in which children and adolescents develop themselves. Examples of such are the different societies in which important knowledge and practices of medical systems are learned early on by children and teenagers [39, 64]. This suggests the role of adult facilitation in the cultural learning of valuable information. Notably, there is also the deliberate action by local healers of not sharing all the crucial information they know about their medicinal skills with younger healers. This is done to maintain their status as healer gurus who “know about all plants,” and, in doing so, they seek prestige and future consultations [65].

If we consider the evolutionary point of view of the acquisition of cultural knowledge and practices, compared to our phylogenetic cousin primates, humans develop the competencies of cultural skills long after weaning and the age of first reproduction [66]. This means that, compared with our relatives the primates, we have a very long childhood. This phenomenon is very costly from an energy perspective, as it depends on extensive support in food and care from several other members of the society [relatives or not] [66]. Nevertheless, it is seen in the literature that it is fundamental to allow long periods of social learning about the numerous codes and signs within the culture. This creates more episodes between the search for testing hypotheses, innovations, and learning to imitate the ability of adults in survival and reproduction [67].

The above-mentioned discussion shows that cultural learning in childhood goes through different stages in its development. On the one hand, it is heavily influenced by adult references who are relatives, as well as by pre-established rules of gender and social dynamics in places where children and adolescents grow up [63, 68, 69]. On the other hand, they have the autonomy to actively search for solutions and acquire knowledge about things in the environment to share with their peers between games and other joint activities [62]. Our results on the non-similarity of important medicinal plants between children and adults in the studied community may reflect a child’s long process of acquiring knowledge and cultural practices. This process is marked by questions of trial-and-error, testing of hypotheses, and imitation, which is characteristic of this phase of development. These results may also relate the factors related to the act of children sharing aspects of a culture [67, 69]. In children culture, the factors for considering the importance of a medicinal plant may have nothing to do with those considered by adults.

As a brief case study, of the plants contained in the structural core of children and adolescents in our community, only two medicinal plants were categorized as those that are a part of the children’s structural core; these are “ameixa” and “capim santo” (lemongrass). “Ameixa” is the medicinal plant with the highest importance among adults, both in 2017 and 2019 if we consider our entire sample. It is also the one with the highest importance among the adults residing only in Muquém, which would explain why it also appears as the plant with the highest importance value among children. However, “capim santo” is the 24th most important medicinal plant among the adults of Muquém, which falls far from the first nine that make up its structural core (Table 2). The explanation for this pattern may be related to the fact that this medicinal plant is exotic, taken as a tea, and is popularly known in the region for the treatment of stomach complications, especially for children as it is considered “weaker.” Perhaps, the treatment events or perceived efficacy of these two plants are more common in communities related to children, especially in the administration of “capim santo” tea.

Limitations and final considerations

Social-ecological and adaptive evolution is complex because these systems are a direct result of the cognitive, socio-cultural, and environmental/ecological systems of other complexes [52]. Our premises suggest that the popularity of important medicinal plants, measured by salience (structural core), is an adaptive pattern directly related to the simultaneous interaction of the cognitive (adaptive memory), social-ecological [prototypicality], and ecological availability system factors, adjusted to the short-term solution of the treatment of local diseases. It is also important to note that the free list is a method of collecting ethnobiological data that sometimes depends on the context in which the tool was used (see [70]). Our results also point in this direction. In light of this, future studies should gather information about the epidemiological contexts of communities. They should also analyze the role of other related variables, such as organoleptic properties, and relate them to tracking the importance of medicinal plants over space and time to advance the discussion above. Furthermore, ethnobiology and ethnobotany could carry out more longitudinal tracking analyses through the space and time of those variables and verify other factors related to the knowledge of children, such as age, gender, and diseases treated daily. This would be done with the aim of understanding this phenomenon of importance since the beginning of human development.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Alencar NL, Santoro FR, Albuquerque UP. What is the role of exotic medicinal plants in local medical systems? a study from the perspective of utilitarian redundancy. Rev Bras. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bjp.2014.09.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Ferreira Júnior WS, Albuquerque UP. “Consensus Within Diversity”: an evolutionary perspective on local medical systems. Biol Theory. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-015-0215-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Hart BL. The evolution of herbal medicine: behavioural perspectives. Anim Behav. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Saslis-Lagoudakis CH, Hawkins JA, Greenhill SJ, Pendry CA, Watson MF, Tuladhar-Douglas W, Baral SR, Savolainen V. The evolution of traditional knowledge: environment shapes medicinal plant use in Nepal. Proc R Soc B Bio Sci. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Casagrande DG. Human taste and cognition in Tzeltal Maya medicinal plant use. J Ecol Anthropol. 2000;4:57–69.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Thomas E, Vandebroek I, Sanca S, Van Damme P. Cultural significance of medicinal plant families and species among Quechua farmers in Apillapampa. Bolivia J Ethnopharmacol. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jep.2008.11.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Thomas E, Semo L, Morales M, Noza Z, Nunez H, Cayuba A, Noza M, Humaday N, Vaya J, Van Damme P. Ethnomedicinal practices and medicinal plant knowledge of the Yuracarés and Trinitarios from Indigenous Territory and National Park Isiboro-Sécure, Bolivian Amazon. J Ethnopharmacol. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jep.2010.09.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ferreira Júnior WS, Ladio AH, Albuquerque UP. Resilience and adaptation in the use of medicinal plants with suspected anti-inflammatory activity in the Brazilian Northeast. J Ethnopharmacol. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jep.2011.09.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Bernard HR. Research methods in anthropology. Qualitative and quantitative approaches. 4th ed. Walnut Creek: Altamira Press; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Quinlan M. Considerations for collecting freelists in the field: examples from Ethnobotany. Field Methods. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822X05277460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Bonifácio KM, Freire EMX, Schiavetti A. Cultural keystone species of fauna as a method for assessing conservation priorities in a Protected Area of the Brazilian semiarid. Biota Neotrop. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1590/1676-0611-BN-2014-0106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Albuquerque UP, Cunha LVFC, Lucena RFP, Alves RRDN. Methods and techniques in ethnobiology and ethnoecology. 2nd ed. New York: Springer; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Lucena RFP, Medeiros PM, Araújo EL, Alves AGC, Albuquerque UP. The ecological apparency hypothesis and the importance of useful plants in rural communities from Northeastern Brazil: an assessment based on use value. J Environ Manage. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2011.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Medeiros PM, Pinto BLS, Nascimento VT. Can organoleptic properties explain the differential use of medicinal plants? evidence from Northeastern Brazil. J Ethnopharmacol. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jep.2014.11.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Heinrich M, Ankli A, Frei B, Weimann C, Sticher O. Medicinal plants in Mexico: Healers’ consensus and cultural importance. Soc Sci Med. 1998. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536[98]00181-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Turner NJ. “The Importance of a Rose”: evaluating the cultural significance of plants in thompson and lillooet interior salish. Am Anthropol. 1988. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1988.90.2.02a00020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Zarger RK. Children’s ethnoecological knowledge: Situated learning and the cultural transmission of subsintence knowledge and skills among Q’eqchi’ Maya. In B.A., Wake Forest University. 2002. https://getd.libs.uga.edu/pdfs/zarger_rebecca_k_200208_phd.pdf. Accessed 16 Jan 2020.

  18. Hunn E. The utilitarian factor in folk biological classification. Am Anthropol. 1982;84:830–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Atran S, Medin D. The native mind and the cultural construction of nature. 1st ed. Cambridge: The MIT press; 2008.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  20. D’Andrade R. The development of cognitive anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Medin DL, Atran S. The native mind: biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures. Psychol Rev. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.960.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Meireles MPA, Albuquerque UP, Medeiros PM. What interferes with conducting free lists? a comparative ethnobotanical experiment. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2021;17:1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Sousa DCP, Soldati GT, Monteiro JM, Araújo TAS, Albuquerque UP. Information Retrieval during free listing is biased by memory: evidence from medicinal plants. PLoS ONE. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165838.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Ferreira Júnior WS, Medeiros PM, Albuquerque UP. Evolutionary Ethnobiology. eLS. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd: Chichester. 2019; https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470015902.a0028232.

  25. Albuquerque UP, Medeiros PM, Ferreira Júnior WS, Silva TC, Silva RRV, Gonçalves-Souza T. Social-ecological theory of maximization: basic concepts and two initial models. Biol Theory. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-019-00316-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Nairne JS, Thompson SR, Pandeirada JNS. Adaptive memory: survival processing enhances retention. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2007. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Nairne JS, Pandeirada JNS, Gregory KJ, Van Arsdall JE. Adaptive memory: fitness relevance and the hunter-gatherer mind. Psychol Sci. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02356.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Nairne JS, Pandeirada JNS. Adaptive memory: ancestral priorities and the mnemonic value of survival processing. Cogn Psychol. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.01.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. VanArsdall JE, Nairne JS, Pandeirada JNS, Blunt JR. Adaptive memory: animacy processing produces mnemonic advantages. Exp Psychol. 2013;60:172–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Casagrande DG. Ecology, cognition, and cultural transmission of Tzeltal maya medicinal plant knowlegde. In: Lehigh University Database. University of Georgia. 2002. https://www.lehigh.edu/~dac511/pdfs/casagrande.diss.pdf. Accessed 11 Nov 2020.

  31. Mesoudi A, Thornton A. What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Miton H, Charbonneau M. Cumulative culture in the laboratory: methodological and theoretical challenges. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Miu E, Gulley N, Laland KN, Rendell L. Innovation and cumulative culture through tweaks and leaps in online programming contests. Nat Commun. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04494-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos Trans R Socy B: Biol Sci. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. ICMBio. Instituto Chico Mendes de Conservação da Biodiversidade. 2016. http://www.icmbio.gov.br/portal/unidadesdeconservacao/biomas-brasileiros/caatinga/unidades-de-conservacao-caatinga/2135-parna-do-catimbau. Accessed 20 Feb 2020.

  37. Specht MJ, Santos BA, Marshall N, Melo FPL, Leal IR, Tabarelli M, Baldauf C. Socioeconomic differences among resident, users and neighbour populations of a protected area in the Brazilian dry forest. J Environ Manage. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2018.11.101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Silva TLL, Ferreira Júnior WS, Albuquerque UP. Is there a biological basis in the selection of medicinal plants in the human species? an initial approach based on chemosensory perception of taste. Ethnobiol Conserv. 2020;9:1–15.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Quinlan MB, Quinlan RJ, Council SK, Roulette JW. Children’s acquisition of ethnobotanical knowledge in a caribbean horticultural village. J Ethnobiol. 2016. https://doi.org/10.2993/0278-0771-36.2.433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Chaves LS, Nascimento ALB, Albuquerque UP. What matters in free listing? A probabilistic interpretation of the salience index. Acta Bot Brasilica. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1590/0102-33062018abb0330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Anderson MJ, Walsh DC. PERMANOVA, ANOSIM, and the Mantel test in the face of heterogeneous dispersions: what null hypothesis are you testing? Ecol Monogr. 2013;83:557–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. In: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria; 2018.

  43. Oksanen J, Blanchet FG, Friendly M, Kindt R, Al E. vegan: Community Ecology Package. 2018. https://cran.r-project.org/package=vegan. Accessed 13 Mar 2021.

  44. Gonçalves PHS, Albuquerque UP, Medeiros PM. The most commonly available woody plant species are the most useful for human populations: a meta-analysis. Ecol Appl. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1002/eap.1364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Thomas E, Vandebroek I, Goetghebeur P, Sanca S, Arrázola S, Van Damme P. The relationship between plant use and plant diversity in the Bolivian Andes, with special reference to medicinal plant use. Hum Ecol. 2008. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10745-008-9208-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Santos CS, Barros FN, Paula M, Rando J, Nascimento VT, Medeiros PM. What matters when prioritizing a medicinal plant? a study of local criteria for their differential use. Acta Botanica Brasilica. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1590/0102-33062017abb0336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Clement YN, Morton-Gittens J, Basdeo L, Blades A, Francis M, Gomes N, Janjua M, Singh A. Perceived efficacy of herbal remedies by users accessing primary healthcare in Trinidad. BMC Complement Altern Med. 2007. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6882-7-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Reinaldo RCPDS, Santiago ACP, Medeiros PM, Albuquerque UP. Do ferns and lycophytes function as medicinal plants? a study of their low representation in traditional pharmacopoeias. J Ethnopharmacol. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jep.2015.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Berlin B. Ethnobiological classification. Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1992.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  50. Schniter E, Macfarlan SJ, Garcia JJ, Ruiz-Campos G, Beltran DG, Bowen BB, Lerback JC. Age-appropriate wisdom? ethnobiological knowledge ontogeny in Pastoralist Mexican Choyeros. Hum Nat. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09387-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Reyes-García V, Huanca T, Vadez V, Leonard W, Wilkie D. Cultural, practical, and economic value of wild plants: a quantitative study in the Bolivian Amazon. Econ Bot. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1663/0013-0001(2006)60(62:CPAEVO)2.0.CO;2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Albuquerque U.P., Ludwig D., Feitosa I.S., Moura J.M.B., Medeiros P.M., Gonçalves P.H.S., Silva R.H., Silva T.C., Gonçalves-Souza T., FerreiraJúnior W.S. (2020) Addressing Social-Ecological Systems across Temporal and Spatial Scales: a Conceptual Synthesis for Ethnobiology. Human Ecology; Doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10745-020-00189-7.

  53. Nascimento ALB, Lozano A, Melo JG, Alves RRN, Albuquerque UP. Functional aspects of the use of plants and animals in local medical systems and their implications for resilience. J Ethnopharmacol. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jep.2016.08.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Santoro FR, Ferreira Júnior WS, Araújo TAS, Ladio AH, Albuquerque UP. Does plant species richness guarantee the resilience of local medical systems? a perspective from utilitarian redundancy. PLoS ONE. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119826.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Silva RH, Ferreira Júnior WS, Medeiros PM, Albuquerque UP. Adaptive memory and evolution of the human naturalistic mind: insights from the use of medicinal plants. PLoS ONE. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214300.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Ferreira Junior WS, Alencar NL, Albuquerque UP. Methods for data collection in medical ethnobiology. In: Albuquerque UP, Cunha LVFC, Lucena RFP, Alves RRN, editors. Methods and techniques in ethnobiology and ethnoecology. Springer: New York; 2019. p. 111–21.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Gallois S, Reyes-García V. Children and ethnobiology. J Ethnobiol. 2018. https://doi.org/10.2993/0278-0771-38.2.155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Gallois S, Duda R, Reyes-García V. Local ecological knowledge among Baka children: a case of children’s culture? J Ethnobiol. 2017. https://doi.org/10.2993/0278-0771-37.1.60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Schneider W, Ornstein PA. The Development of Children’s Memory. Child Dev Perspect. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Goswami U. The Wiley-Blackwell Hanbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. 2nd ed. Wiley-Blackwell Handbooks of Developmental Psychology; 2011.

  61. Tian X. Generating pastoral skills through work and play in the daily life of Maasai Children in Kenya. J Ethnobiol. 2018;38:170–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Gallois S, Duda R, Hewlett B, Reyes-García V. Children’s daily activities and knowledge acquisition: a case study among the Baka from southeastern Cameroon. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-015-0072-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Bruschi P, Sugni M, Moretti A, Signorini MA, Fico G. Children’s versus adult’s knowledge of medicinal plants: an ethnobotanical study in Tremezzina [Como, Lombardy, Italy]. Rev Bras. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bjp.2019.04.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Geissler PW, Harris SA, Prince RJ, Olsen A, Odhiambo RA, Oketch-Rabah H, Madiega PA, Andersen A, Mølgaard P. Medicinal plants used by Luo mothers and children in Bondo district, Kenya. J Ethnopharmacol. 2002. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-8741[02]00191-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Shukla S, Sinclair AJ. Becoming a traditional medicinal plant healer: Divergent views of practicing and young healers on traditional medicinal plant knowledge skills in India. Ethnobot Res Appl. 2009;7:39–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Schuppli C, Isler K, Van Schaik CP. How to explain the unusually late age at skill competence among humans. J Hum Evol. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2012.08.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Gopnik A, O’Grady S, Lucas CG, Griffiths TL, Wente A, Bridgers S, Aboody R, Fung H, Dahl RE. Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1700811114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Gallois S, Lubbers MJ, Hewlett B, Reyes-García V. Social networks and knowledge transmission strategies among Baka children southeastern Cameroon. Hum Nat. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9328-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Porcher V, Carrière SM, Gallois S, Randriambanona H, Rafidison VM, Reyes-García V. Growing up in the Betsileo landscape: Children’s wild edible plants knowledge in Madagascar. PLoS ONE. 2022;17:1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Zambrana NYP, Bussmann RW, Hart RE, Huanca ALM, Soria GO, Vaca MO, et al. To list or not to list? the value and detriment of freelisting in ethnobotanical studies. Nat Plants. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41477-018-0128-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the members of the Laboratory of Ecology and Evolution of Social-Ecological Systems (Laboratório de Ecologia e Evolução de Sistemas Socioecológicos—LEA) for their contributions to this study. We would also like to thank Prof. Dr. Thiago Gonçalves Souza (UFRPE) and biologist Danilo Vicente Oliveira for their statistical support and all of the residents of Igrejinha, Muquém, Dor de Dente, Túnel, and Açude Velho who welcomed us so well on all occasions, especially Dona Socorro and Seu Rivaldo, who provided us with all the logistical support for the field activities.

Funding

This study was financed by the FACEPE [Foundation for Support to Science and Technology of the State of Pernambuco] [http://www.facepe.br] awarded to DCPS, and CNPq [National Council for Scientific and Technological Development] in the productivity grant awarded to UPA. The study received a contribution from the INCT Ethnobiology, Bioprospecting, and Nature Conservation, certified by CNPq, with financial support from the Foundation for Support to Science and Technology of the State of Pernambuco as a grant [Grant number: APQ-0562-2.01/17] given to UPA. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or in manuscript preparation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

UPA, WSFJ, and DCPS contributed to conceptualization; UPA and DCPS were involved in formal analysis; UPA contributed to funding acquisition; DCPS was involved in investigation; DCPS, UPA, and WSFJ contributed to methodology and writing—review and editing; UPA was involved in project administration; UPA contributed to resources; UPA and WSFJ were involved in supervision; and DCPS contributed to writing—original draft. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ulysses Paulino Albuquerque.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was submitted and approved by the Official State Body of the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Pernambuco (CAAE:89887817.6.0000.5207).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Raw data of the hypothesis 1.

Additional file 2.

Raw data of the hypothesis 2.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sousa, D.C.P., Ferreira Júnior, W.S. & Albuquerque, U.P. Short-term temporal analysis and children's knowledge of the composition of important medicinal plants: the structural core hypothesis. J Ethnobiology Ethnomedicine 18, 51 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-022-00548-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-022-00548-2

Keywords