Skip to main content

The ethnoprimatology of the Maijuna of the Peruvian Amazon and implications for primate conservation

Abstract

Background

In Amazonia, primates are not only an important food source but they also hold significant cultural and symbolic value for many indigenous groups. We document the relationship between primates and community members of the Maijuna indigenous community of Sucusari in the Peruvian Amazon and describe how ethnoprimatological studies provide a better understanding of the significance of primates in people’s lives. Additionally, we explore how ethnoprimatological studies can help inform and enhance primate conservation initiatives.

Methods

Semi-structured interviews were conducted with 50 residents of the community of Sucusari to assess the classification, cultural significance and traditional uses, beliefs, ceremonies and stories of primates within the Sucusari River basin.

Results

Primates play an important role in the lives of individuals in the Sucusari community. They are distinguished by their arboreal lifestyle, and among the 11 species reported in the area, seven (Lagothrix lagotricha, Alouatta seniculus, Pithecia monachus, Callicebus spp., Saimiri sciureus, Leontocebus nigricollis) are highly recognized and culturally salient. Primates are used as food, medicine, pets, domestic tools and in the production of handicrafts. They are primarily hunted for local consumption, with larger primates such as L. lagotricha being preferred. Lagothrix lagotricha was also the most commonly reported pet species and the only observed pet primate in the community during surveys. Maijuna traditional beliefs include ancestral dietary taboos for A. seniculus, which are referred to as sorcerer monkeys, but this taboo is no longer fully adhered to. Maijuna traditional stories associated with primates describe the origin of primates found in Sucusari.

Conclusion

Primates are embedded in the intricate sociocultural system of the community of Sucusari. Better understanding the relationship between primates and people can help to focus conservation efforts on primate species of particularly high sociocultural importance as well as ecological value, such as L. lagotricha. We highly recommend the inclusion of ethnoprimatological studies into primate conservation initiatives to accomplish more effective conservation planning, ultimately integrating the goals of biodiversity conservation with the cultural and economic needs of indigenous and local communities.

Background

Neotropical forests contain the highest diversity of primate fauna in the world with 171 out of 504 primate species listed in the most recent taxonomic compilation, followed by Asia (119 species), Africa (111 species), and Madagascar (103 species) [1]. Forest sites in Amazonia typically contain ten or more sympatric monkey species, comprising a large part of the local arboreal vertebrate biomass [2, 3]. Moreover, they are important seed dispersers and predators [4], playing a key role in forest dynamics [5,6,7,8]. Amazonian primate populations are threatened by deforestation and forest degradation [9,10,11,12] and by widespread hunting [13, 14].

Primate populations and their fates are inevitably linked to the activities of resident human populations. While a number of studies examine the effects of human activities on primate populations [15,16,17], few studies in Amazonia have recognized the importance of studying the interconnection between humans and other primates, from the perspective of local human residents [18,19,20,21]. Ethnozoology has become an important field that is focused on the interactions between humans and animals encompassing a wide array of studies. Ethnozoological research has demonstrated that human populations possess extensive ecological knowledge about animals that can complement western scientific knowledge as well as provide valuable data for conservation [22]. Ethnoprimatology, an important subdiscipline of ethnozoology, is focused on the interactions between humans and non-human primates where humans are considered an integral part of primate ecosystems [23]. Some ethnoprimatological studies in the Amazon have demonstrated the significant role of primates in the lives of indigenous people [18,19,20,21, 24, 25], encompassing subsistence hunting, food preference and avoidance, beliefs, ceremonies, and stories that define the symbolism and identity of indigenous groups. Primates are therefore embedded within complex sociocultural relationships, where human hunting, traditional uses, beliefs, and myths affect primate ecology. Hence, ethnoprimatological studies provide valuable insights and information regarding the cultural significance of primates to indigenous peoples that directly effects the development and implementation of sustainable management practices and conservation strategies.

The Maijuna (also known as the Orejón or Coto) are a Western Tucanoan people who live in the northeastern Peruvian Amazon [26]. Studies on the traditional knowledge of the Maijuna have focused on ethnobotanical information [26,27,28], yet no research has been conducted in regards to the sociocultural importance of primate populations to the Maijuna indigenous group. Currently, less than 500 Maijuna individuals live within Maijuna ancestral territory, distributed in four communities: Puerto Huamán and Nueva Vida along the Yanayacu River, San Pablo de Totolla (Totoya) along the Algodón River, and Sucusari along the Sucusari River (Fig. 1). Each community is recognized as a native community (Comunidad Nativa) by the Peruvian government and has been granted title to the land surrounding their community [29].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study area—the Maijuna community of Sucusari, Loreto, Peru

Over the years, Maijuna ancestral lands have been subjected to many illegal and unsustainable activities by outsiders causing an overexploitation of natural resources and negatively impacting local livelihood strategies [30]. In 2006, the Maijuna decided to push for the formal protection of their lands in the form of an official protected area to prevent further degradation of their territory and biocultural resources. Since then, the Maijuna have increased their interest in developing sustainable activities to protect their natural resources [31]. In June of 2015, after almost 10 years of struggle by the Maijuna and their allies, the National Government of Peru formally created the Maijuna-Kichwa Regional Conservation Area (MKRCA). The Decree No. 008-2015 of the Ministry of Environment officially protects 391,039 ha of Maijuna ancestral territory to conserve its vast biodiversity and support local livelihoods [32]. Thus, identifying and understanding the main drivers of anthropogenic pressures on wildlife populations is critically important to develop effective management plans in the new MKRCA. Within this framework, the present study aims to document and understand the significance and relationship between primates and community members of the Maijuna indigenous community of Sucusari. Specifically, it described and assessed the cultural significance and traditional uses, beliefs, ceremonies and stories of primates in the Sucusari River basin, with the ultimate aim to better inform primate conservation.

Methods

Study area

This study was conducted in the Maijuna indigenous community of Sucusari, situated along the Sucusari River, a tributary of the Napo River, in northeastern Peru (Fig. 1). The community is located approximately 126 km by river from Iquitos, the capital of the Department of Loreto [27]. The Sucusari community has legal title to 4771 ha, which adjoins the recently established MKRCA, and is the only village located within the Sucusari River basin. An ecotourism lodge, established in 1983, is located approximately 4.5 km downriver from the main community [33].

The community has 166 residents divided into 32 monofamilial or plurifamilial houses. Within the total number of inhabitants of the Sucusari community, 59% are Maijuna, 35% are mestizos,Footnote 1 and the remaining 6% are Kichwa. The main subsistence activities of community members include hunting, fishing, swidden-fallow agriculture, and the gathering of various forest products [30]. To generate income, residents sell game meat, domesticated animals, agricultural products, and a variety of non-timber forest products [30].

The MKRCA harbors a vast area of intact rain forest, encompassing the headwaters of seven rivers (one of which is the Sucusari River) that supply two large Amazonian watersheds (i.e., Napo and Putumayo). The area also contains a complex of high terraces—a previously unknown habitat—that shelters a flora and fauna with a number of new, rare, and specialized species [32]. The MKRCA harbors biological resources that are vital for the survival of local people, including non-timber forest products (e.g., Mauritia flexuosa palm fruits), birds, tortoises, fish (e.g., Arapaima gigas, Osteoglossum bicirrhosum), and large mammals such as primates [32]. Within the study area, 11 primate species have been previously reported in the literature [30, 34,35,36] (Table 1).

Table 1 Primate species reported in the Sucusari River basin or surrounding areas, Napo River, Peru

Data collection

We interviewed 50 residents of the Sucusari community in August 2014, targeting both male and female adults (> 18 years old) that were available and willing to be interviewed. The sample included 30 (60%) males and 20 (40%) females, with a mean age of 43.58 (SD ± 13.82) years (Table 2). For the purposes of this project, hunters were defined as any individual of the community that has hunted once or more during their lifetime. Individual interviews were conducted in Spanish or in Maijuna, the latter with the help of a community leader that is fluent in both languages. Before beginning the interviews, prior informed consent (PIC) was obtained from the Sucusari community as well as from individual research participants [37].

Table 2 Demographics of the interviewees from the Maijuna community of Sucusari, Loreto, Peru

Individuals were interviewed using a semi-structured questionnaire [38] to gather sociocultural information (i.e., uses, beliefs, ceremonies, and stories) for all primate species found within the Sucusari River basin. The beginning of the questionnaire contained basic demographic questions and then shifted to questions focused on how individuals define and classify monkeys. This was then followed up by a freelisting exercise to determine the cultural salience of the primate species found in the area. To verify local knowledge, the questionnaire then included photographs of 20 primate species, nine of which do not occur in the area serving as a control. The control species were Saguinus mystax [39], Leontocebus tripartitus [39], Aotus nigriceps [40], Callicebus cupreus [41], Saimiri boliviensis [42], Cacajao calvus [43], Ateles belzebuth [44], Ateles chamek [44], and Lagothrix poeppigii [42]. The remaining 11 photographs were of the species reported to occur in the area [30, 34,35,36] and were used to validate the supposition that people were aware of the species in their area, rather than guessing. The 11 primate species were: Cebuella pygmaea [42], Leontocebus nigricollis [39], Aotus vociferans [40], Saimiri sciureus [42], Callicebus discolor [41], Callicebus lucifer [41], Pithecia monachus [45], Cebus albifrons [42], Sapajus macrocephalus [46], Alouatta seniculus [42], and Lagothrix lagotricha [42]. Questions regarding the use, beliefs, ceremonies, and stories for the different primate species reported in the area were then only asked when individuals were able to correctly identify the primate as well as correctly indicate its occurrence in the area (hereafter described as the “correct location”).

Data analysis—freelisting and cultural salience

Freelisting is a method that can be used to determine the cultural salience of named species, representing the cultural importance of a particular animal within the studied community [43]. Freelisting was used at the beginning of interviews to determine the cultural salience of the primate species found in the area. Cultural salience calculations assume that (1) items named by most individuals are more salient and (2) the first items in the list are more salient [47]. Based on these assumptions, Quinlan [48] created the following formula to determine the cultural salience of each animal species listed by an individual:

Salience = (1 + length i  − position i )/lengthi, where length is the total number of animals listed by individual i and position is the location of a particular animal in the list of individual i. Animals not listed by an individual had a cultural salience of zero.

The cultural salience of each animal was calculated using the following equation:

Cultural salience = Σ salience i /n, where: n (n = 50) is the total number of respondents who participated in the study.

Results

Defining and classifying monkeys

In order to elucidate how primates are locally defined and classified within the Sucusari community, interviewees were asked to define the term mono in Spanish, which means monkey in English. There was no clear consensus of the definition and classification of primates. Responses given were coded and divided into eight categories: (1) live in trees (68%), (2) jump/climb on branches (52%), (3) eat fruits/food in trees (32%), (4) possess a long tail (32%), (5) possess a prehensile tail (12%), (6) can go down to the ground to eat (10%), (7) distinct fur (4%), and (8) live in various places (2%). Most of these qualities also apply to some of the non-primate species listed by respondents during freelisting, such as Procyonidae Potos flavus.

Freelisting and cultural salience

During the beginning of the interviews, individuals were asked to freelist all of the monkey species that are present in the Sucusari River basin. The interviewees listed 11 primate species in total. Five species were mentioned by more than 80% of respondents: Pithecia monachus (96%), Saimiri sciureus (94%), Alouatta seniculus (92%), Lagothrix lagotricha (90%), and Leontocebus nigricollis (84%). Additional species listed were Cebus albifrons (72%), Aotus vociferans (50%), Cebuella pygmaea (40%), and Sapajus macrocephalus (10%). The titi monkeys (Callicebus discolor and Callicebus lucifer) represented a unique case. Although 92% of the respondents mentioned titi monkeys during the freelisting, 61% referred to them with a single common name (tocón) and, therefore, did not differentiate between the two species. It is also worth noting that during freelisting, 22% of respondents listed only primate species whereas 78% of them also mentioned other non-primate mammal species (Table 3). The kinkajou (Potos flavus), a nocturnal mammal species with many features that resemble a primate, was mentioned by 64% of all respondents.

Table 3 Non-primate mammal species reported as monkeys during freelisting

Both primate and non-primate mammal species mentioned during the freelisting were included to calculate the cultural salience of the animals listed. Analysis showed that the most culturally salient primate species were S. sciureus (0.68), P. monachus (0.66), Callicebus spp. (0.60), L. lagotricha (0.55), A. seniculus (0.52), and L. nigricollis (0.52). The species with lower cultural salience were C. albifrons (0.40), A. vociferans (0.20), C. pygmaea (0.18), and S. macrocephalus (0.04) (Fig. 2). Given that 61% of interviewees referred to the titi monkeys using a single common name (tocón) and did not differentiate between the two species (C. discolor and C. lucifer), the single common name was used for the analysis (Callicebus spp.). P. flavus had the highest cultural salience (0.30) among non-primate mammal species, even greater than some primate species.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Cultural salience of all primate and non-primate species listed by interviewees during freelisting. Darker and lighter bars are primate and non-primate mammal species, respectively. Cultural salience ranges from 0 to 1, with 1 being the most culturally salient

It is important to note that members of the Sucusari community can be divided into two main groups based on ethnicity: Maijuna and non-Maijuna individuals, the latter including mestizos and Kichwa individuals. Indigenous and non-indigenous people have different cultural perceptions, traditions, and views regarding the environment that surrounds them [49, 50]. For this reason, the cultural salience of freelisted species was calculated for each group separately (Fig. 3a, b). Seven primate species (S. sciureus, P. monachus, Callicebus spp., L. nigricollis, L. lagotricha, and A. seniculus) had the highest cultural salience for both groups. However, for the Maijuna, L. lagotricha had a cultural salience of 0.64, almost 20% higher than for non-Maijuna respondents (0.46).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Cultural salience of primate and non-primate species for Maijuna (a) and non-Maijuna (b) interviewees. Darker and lighter bars are primate and non-primate mammal species, respectively. Cultural salience ranges from 0 to 1, with 1 being the most culturally salient

Photo identification exercise

Seven out of the 11 primate species found within the Sucusari River basin were correctly identified during the photo exercise by more than 75% of the interviewees (Fig. 4a). These species include L. lagotricha (96%), P. monachus (96%), C. discolor (90%), L. nigricollis (90%), S. sciureus (88%), A. seniculus (86%), and C. lucifer (78%). These were also the primate species with the highest cultural salience (Figs. 2 and 3). In regards to indicating the correct location of each primate species, respondents who correctly identified the primate species from a photo were more likely to correctly identify its occurrence in the area. The exception to this was S. macrocephalus, which was considered to be present in the Sucusari River basin by only 8% of the respondents (Fig. 4a). It is worth noting that the species that were most commonly confused by the respondents during the photo exercise were S. macrocephalus and C. albifrons, and they had the third (62%) and fourth (68%) lowest rates of identification by interviewees, respectively. The two species with the lowest identification rates were A. vociferans (30%), which is the only nocturnal primate in the area, and C. pygmaea (56%), which is the smallest primate of the group and cryptically colored. As expected, the control species were not recognized by the majority of interviewees, except for L. poepiggi, identified as choro colorado in Spanish or red woolly monkey in English, and A. chamek, identified as mono araña in Spanish or spider monkey in English, by 60 and 52% of the interviewees respectively (Fig. 4b). These two species are some of the largest primates and highly consumed by Amazonian indigenous peoples.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Percentages of correct identification and location of primates reported along the Sucusari River (a) and control species (b). Correct identification (darker bars) and location (lighter bars) of primates were assessed through the photo identification exercise

Traditional uses of primates

Interviewees that correctly identified a primate in the photo exercise as well as correctly indicated its occurrence in the Sucusari River basin were asked to list all the known uses for that primate species. Among all responses given, primates are used as food, medicine, pets, and domestic tools (e.g., tails used as dusters to clean houses) and in the production of handicrafts (Table 4). Although it was not part of the questionnaire, some respondents provided further information regarding primate food preparation, and this information was augmented through direct observation. After primates are killed by hunters using shotguns, their fur is burned off over an open fire, constantly being flipped to prevent burning. The entrails of the monkey are then removed and it is butchered. The meat is then cooked by grilling, boiling, or smoking, and all edible parts are then consumed. No information on specific primate species or body parts consumed by specific members of the populace was indicated nor observed, and detailed recipes were not obtained through the questionnaire. However, three respondents reported that L. lagotricha is considered the preferred primate species to hunt, not only for its larger size but also for its flavor. Hunting and eating woolly monkeys (L. lagotricha) is especially preferred during the fruiting season of leche huayo (Couma macrocarpa), also called leche caspi, due to their increased body fat content during that period.

Table 4 Traditional Maijuna and Kichwa uses, beliefs, ceremonies, and stories for the primates of Sucusari, Loreto, Peru

Although A. seniculus is one of the largest primates and had been eaten at some point by all of the respondents, it was not described as “tasty” by any of the interviewees, except one mestizo man. In the Maijuna culture, red howlers were infrequently eaten in the past due to food taboos or avoidance, as it was considered a sorcerer (see below). P. monachus—a medium-sized monkey—was reported as very tasty by one Maijuna interviewee. Though medium-sized and small monkey species are eaten (except for C. pygmaea), these are not frequently hunted due to their size. Many respondents stated that it is not worth using shotgun shells (due to their cost) on small primates given the limited meat obtained. Nonetheless, L. nigricollis and A. vociferans are hunted in times of game scarcity—a situation that did not occur in the area at the time of study. It is important to highlight that all of the male interviewees had hunted at some point in their lives.

All 11 primate species are occasionally raised as pets. However, respondents indicated preferences as well. A. seniculus (67%) and C. albifrons (78%) were two of the primate species with the lowest percentages of positive responses. A. seniculus are rarely kept as a pet because their loud calls disturb household members, and C. albifrons is considered mischievous and restless, making them difficult to keep as pets. L. nigricollis and S. sciureus were reported to be beneficial as pets given that they help to keep the house free of insects due to their insectivorous behavior. Among all the primate species, L. lagotricha was the most commonly reported as being kept as a pet (98%) and the only observed primate pet in the community during the study.

The tails of P. monachus, C. discolor, and C. lucifer were reported as being used as house dusters, especially those from P. monachus (48%) because of their bushy nature. Bones and teeth of S. sciureus, C. discolor, P. monachus, C. albifrons, and A. seniculus are used in handicrafts. The hyoid bone of A. seniculus, one of the most salient features of the red howler monkey, are occasionally used as a cup to drink beverages and in handicrafts. Furthermore, one male Maijuna respondent reported the use of the hyoid bone of A. seniculus to treat sore throats. According to this individual, water is poured into the hyoid bone, kept there during sunset, and ultimately consumed as a remedy. Individuals who take this remedy cannot eat red howler monkey meat anymore because they have used this primate to treat their illness. One Kichwa interviewee reported the use of the tail of P. monachus to treat digestive problems, such as diarrhea and vomiting. The tail is burned and then combined with warm water.

Among all of the uses identified, the use of most primates as sources of food and as household pets had a greater importance given a higher number of positive responses. There were no marked differences in the use of primates between Maijuna and non-Maijuna individuals, except for medicinal treatments, fur to cover drums, and the commercialization of meat (Table 4). Commercialization of primate meat (i.e., L. lagotricha and A. seniculus) was only mentioned by two interviewees, possibly indicating that primates are not frequently targeted for this activity.

Traditional beliefs, ceremonies, and stories about primates

Traditional beliefs regarding primates linked to dietary taboos were exclusively found among the Maijuna (Table 4). In the past, Maijuna ancestors believed that red howler monkeys (A. seniculus) were sorcerers, hence the reason why this species is sometimes called mono brujo in Spanish or sorcerer monkey in English (Fig. 5). According to Maijuna traditional beliefs, this monkey harmed children and adults, and its meat was avoided. Although this belief is no longer as strong as it once was, every time A. seniculus meat is consumed, Maijuna individuals believe that it should be done in silence as a sign of respect. In fact, if someone says: “I do not like the meat” or if it is thrown away, it is believed that a tumor may appear in the body or throat of that person, which could lead to death.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Alouatta seniculus, which Maijuna ancestors believed were sorcerers

Lagothrix lagotricha (Fig. 6) is one of the primate species most deeply intertwined in Maijuna traditional culture. Prior to 1974, L. lagotricha was an integral part of the ceremony associated with the first yearly harvest of Bactris gasipaes (pijuayo) palm fruits. Bactris gasipaes is a culturally important domesticated palm, and the fruits are eaten cooked or consumed as a fermented beverage. The ceremony included the ritualistic consumption of woolly monkeys, and the meat of this monkey also served as part of a traditional courtship ritual for the Maijuna. If a woman accepted a piece of woolly monkey meat given by a Maijuna man, then she was also accepting the man, whereas if she declined the offering, it meant that she was not interested in the man and was uninterested in his proposal.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Lagothrix lagotricha, one of the most culturally important primates for the Maijuna

According to the Maijuna, P. monachus is considered poisonous for dogs to eat due to the “poisonous fruits” this monkey eats in the forest. Therefore, after the meat is consumed by people, any leftovers are discarded carefully. In case dogs do get poisoned, their ears are cut in order to expel the poison. The night monkey, Aotus vociferans, was described by some interviewees as a devil monkey that used to eat people, especially hunters in their hunting camps. This belief is related to a local name that some use for this species, which is buri-buri. However, most of the interviewees (58%) had never heard of the name buri-buri. Only 16% of interviewees agreed that buri-buri and the night monkey (A. vociferans) are the same animal, and 26% alleged that they are different species. Differences between the night monkey and buri-buri were difficult for interviewees to explain and describe due to their nocturnal behavior and the lack of visibility at night. The differences identified were mainly explained by their distinctive calls with the buri-buri call being described as “buri, buri, buri, buri.” The few physical differences identified between the two were (1) buri-buri is lighter in color than A. vociferans, (2) they have different color patterns on their foreheads, and (3) buri-buri is larger in size than A. vociferans. Interestingly, one Maijuna interviewee stated that buri-buri is in fact the Kichwa name for A. vociferans. However, this was disputed by the only Kichwa individual interviewed, who stated that the Kichwa name for buri-buri is in fact makuru. He described the buri-buri as a demon monkey that is smaller in size than A. vociferans with a shorter tail, lighter coloration, and yellow lines on its face.

In addition to traditional beliefs about primates, the Maijuna also have traditional stories about them. The large majority of Maijuna traditional stories associated with primates are linked to traditional cosmological beliefs as well as natural history information (e.g., physical attributes, diets, and calls of the different primate species). For the Maijuna, primates descended from humans and were created by Maineno, their traditional Creator, as a means to obtain fruits from trees. Recounting the transformations performed by Maineno helped the Maijuna to make sense of, understand, and narrate the origin of primates present in the Sucusari River basin. The creation of various monkey species and other stories are detailed in the traditional Maijuna stories presented in Appendixes 1, 2, 3, and 4. Interestingly, C. pygmaea, A. vociferans, P. monachus, and S. macrocephalus were not included in any of the Maijuna traditional stories documented as part of this study. Per the only Kichwa individual interviewed for this study, in Kichwa mythology, only A. seniculus and C. discolor have a story that details the physical attributes of both species (Appendix 5).

Discussion

Primates are one of the most hunted taxa in Amazonia and other tropical regions around the world [47, 51, 52]. The close interaction between human and non-human primates has led to increased interest in better understanding people’s perceptions, use, and cultural beliefs of this important group of mammals. For the residents of Sucusari, how they perceive and define monkeys results in the inclusion of other arboreal mammal species beyond just primates, such as kinkajous, which is similar to ethnobiological classification systems in other parts of Amazonia [18, 21, 53, 54]. The arboreal lifestyle of these other mammal species and the fact that they can climb within and jump between trees, some of the key characteristics of Neotropical primates [55], were important factors in grouping them together with primates.

Salience of biological organisms is shaped by the degree of interactions between people and those organisms [56] and could be reflected by its cultural importance and/or its abundance in the environment. In this study, large body size, sociocultural importance, and higher abundance of certain primate species could be critical factors determining cultural salience. Salience results suggested that seven primates were particularly important to interviewees. A first group, comprised of L. lagotricha, A. seniculus, and P. monachus, are all within the top five primate species in regard to body mass in the Sucusari River basin, and they all still play an important role within Maijuna and non-Maijuna culture. A second group comprised of small-bodied primates, is made up of S. sciureus, Callicebus spp. and L. nigricollis. In the Sucusari River basin, S. sciureus is found in large troops (50–100; authors, personal observations) and are frequently observed along riverbanks. Considering that most of the interviewees spend large amounts of time on the river traveling and fishing, the probability of seeing troops of S. sciureus is high. A high cultural salience of Callicebus spp. may be linked to the fact that their calls are frequently heard in the community during the morning (authors, personal observations). And, L. nigricollis is the most abundant primate species within the river basin increasing the probability that interviewees will encounter them [36]. It is worth noting that the cultural salience of primates between Maijuna and non-Maijuna individuals in the community of Sucusari did not show marked differences among species, except for L. lagotricha, which had a salience of 0.64 for the Maijuna compared to 0.46 for non-Maijuna individuals. This result could be due to the close relationship between L. lagotricha and Maijuna cultural traditions, such as the use of L. lagotricha as an important food source and common household pet and previous use as an important ceremonial food by Maijuna ancestors.

Primates represent an important food source for Amazonian indigenous groups, and in some regions, they are considered one of the most delicious mammals [16], especially primates of the subfamily Atelinae due to their greater biomass [13, 19, 57, 58]. In the Sucusari community, primates are hunted primarily for subsistence and rarely for their commercial value (i.e., only L. lagotricha and A. seniculus). Although commercialization of primate meat does not currently represent an important source of income for the community of Sucusari, this should be closely watched given there is a market for game meat in the nearest population centers of Mazán and Iquitos.

Like many other Amazonian indigenous groups, the Maijuna used to hunt with blow guns. However, the transition to a western-style market economy allowed the Maijuna to acquire firearms, thus increasing the probability of the overexploitation of wildlife, including primates. This technology along with food preferences (i.e., particularly L. lagotricha and P. monachus) could have a negative impact on the sustainability of primate hunting in Sucusari, thus highlighting the need to determine sustainable harvest rates for these species. Large-bodied primates are more susceptible to overharvesting than other smaller primates (i.e., Callitrichid primates) due to differences in population density and lower reproductive rates [59]; therefore, they can be highly threatened by subsistence hunting if not managed effectively [14,15,16].

In Amazonia, primate symbolism and subsistence hunting are highly intertwined; therefore, the availability or abundance of a primate is not a direct indicator of its utilization [25]. Maijuna traditional beliefs regarding A. seniculus, considered a sorcerer monkey by Maijuna ancestors, could reflect cultural attitudes against its consumption in comparison to mestizos or Kichwa. In the past, howler monkeys created fear among Maijuna ancestors and ultimately helped to prevent overharvesting of this species. The Matsigenkas [19] and Matsés of Peru [60] as well as the Barí of Venezuela [18] claimed that howler monkeys were also avoided due to its distasteful meat and cultural taboos (i.e., possessors of spiritual hazards). Sadly, the Maijuna in the Sucusari community are currently undergoing rapid loss of their traditional beliefs [28]; thus, taboos against red howler monkey hunting are weakening and have almost disappeared (only two elder Maijuna men mentioned it during this study). Cultural taboos associated with red howler monkeys are an important tool to counteract the effects of subsistence hunting, which could lead to unsustainable harvest yields on howler populations. Cultural beliefs associated with A. vociferans and buri-buri (the demon monkey) remain unclear, creating uncertainty around the origin of this belief. Two main possibilities exist: (1) buri-buri is a non-Maijuna belief that has been learned and incorporated only by some individuals in the community or (2) it is a Maijuna belief that is being lost due to the rapid expansion of westernized culture.

The capture of primate infants as pets is commonly reported in the Amazon as an offshoot of hunting [24, 57]. It is done through seeking female primates with their infants and retrieving them after the mother is shot and falls to the ground. Selective hunting of females may represent a challenge for the sustainability of primate populations and subsistence hunting in the community of Sucusari, leading to skewed sex ratios in primate populations, which is exacerbated by their low reproductive rates [61]. Ateline primates are of particular concern given that they are considered preferred primate prey as well as being reported as favorite pets among several Amazonian indigenous communities [19, 25, 58]. Pet keeping also holds a particular significance for indigenous groups [20]. For the Guajá, keeping primates as pets plays a key role in defining the socialization of children, such as developing infant care for girls and hunting skills for boys [20]. During this study, woolly monkeys were the only primates being kept as pets by one Maijuna woman in the village, who was not able to give birth. The two woolly monkeys were treated like family members. She gave them a traditional alcoholic beverage brewed from cassava, treated them with medicines if they had malaria or other illnesses, and gave them human names. It is worth noting that although only two woolly monkeys were observed as pets during this study, a variety of other primate species have been observed as pets over the years in the Maijuna communities (authors, personal observations).

The wide variety of uses of primates, especially for food, folk medicine, and pet keeping, among Amazonian communities have serious implications for human health [62], which are rarely discussed or taken into account while studying human and non-human primate interactions. Zoonotic diseases such as malaria, dengue, leishmaniasis, leptospirosis, etc. are some of the most common in the Neotropics [63] and are all found in the Sucusari community (authors, personal observations). The consumption of primates for food or medicinal purposes aids in the spread of pathogenic organisms to humans. Thus, special attention should be given while assessing primate meat consumption, pet keeping, or traditional medicines, due to the fact that these species could be potential reservoirs for pathogen transmission, particularly with the increased interest in potential zoonotic malaria. Recently, a study conducted in southern Peru identified two primate hosts (Saguinus imperator and Saguinus fuscicollis) for Plasmodium brasiliensis and Plasmodium malariae, the latter being the one that infects humans [64]. Furthermore, this research suggested and provided evidence that Callitrichids might act as reservoirs for human zoonotic malaria. In the community of Sucusari, efforts to test non-human primates that are in contact with humans for the presence of the protozoan parasite that causes malaria are needed to assess the possibility of zoonoses.

For indigenous people, traditional stories symbolize unity and the creation of shared bonds [65]. Most of these stories document the creation of their lands, disentangle natural events, and/or provide the ethical and moral foundations on which indigenous cultures are built [66, 67]. The traditional stories about primates of the Maijuna and other Amazonian indigenous groups [18, 19] explain their origin through the transformation of human beings into monkeys by their creators. These stories highlight the significance of natural resources for the survival of living beings, reinforcing the connection indigenous people have with nature [68, 69]. The Maijuna monkey creation story is rooted in Maijuna cosmological beliefs and symbolizes the relationship between humans and nonhuman primates, including detailed natural history information.

For the Maijuna, an increase in contact with outsiders and missionaries, a western education system that does not value traditional knowledge, and the entry of mestizos into their communities and their integration into the market economy has led to a disconnect with their cultural practices, causing a rapid loss of their traditional knowledge [28, 29], commonly recognized as acculturation [70]. Unfortunately, in the community of Sucusari, acculturation is manifested in younger generations, where cultural knowledge—especially the Maijuna language and traditional beliefs (e.g., cultural taboos associated with red howler monkeys), ceremonies (e.g., the first yearly harvest of Bactris gasipaes fruits), and stories (e.g., the Maijuna monkey creation story)—is being lost at a rapid rate [28].

The importance of the role of indigenous people in conservation is increasing [71,72,73,74]. This is especially true in the case of the Maijuna given the recent creation of the immense and heavily forested MKRCA. In the community of Sucusari, despite the effects of acculturation on the traditional lives of the Maijuna, community members still remain deeply connected to their ancestral lands and rely heavily on natural resources for both subsistence and income generation. The results of this study highlight that primates are regularly hunted and culturally significant in a wide variety of ways to the community. Additionally, some species hold special cultural value for the Maijuna. All of this information helps to shed light on the ethnoprimatology of the Maijuna and can help to focus conservation efforts on primate species of particularly high sociocultural importance as well as ecological value, such as L. lagotricha. Ultimately, this has implications for working to sustainably manage hunting within the Sucusari river basin and, thus, on the viability of primate populations.

Conclusion

Understanding the sociocultural importance and use of primates by indigenous and local communities should be an essential component of any primatological study and should not be overlooked. This research highlights that primates are regularly hunted and culturally significant in a wide variety of ways to the community of Sucusari. It has shed light on culturally important primate species, particularly for the Maijuna, and how the rapid loss of traditional knowledge, beliefs, and practices may have a negative impact on primate populations and Maijuna cultural identity. Given that biologists frequently ignore the existence of traditional knowledge and beliefs within primatological studies, understanding their significance is critical to identify cultural factors that influence hunter behavior and choice and the concomitant effects on primate populations. Information on what primate species are most frequently hunted and how they are used, what percentage of game meat consists of primates, the evaluation and monitoring of primate hunting levels (including harvest rates), and whether there are zoonotic infections from primates in the community should be a priority to ensure the long-term conservation of primates as well as the protection of human welfare.

Although the Maijuna are interested in the implementation of sustainable management practices, their success will depend on local circumstances and buy-in from the entire community, including non-Maijuna individuals. The community of Sucusari represents the current situation in many Amazonian indigenous communities, where a mix of different ethnic groups inhabits the same land. Therefore, the understanding of sociocultural differences in regards to resource use among different ethnic groups living in a single area is critical for effective community engagement and community-based primate conservation programs. In the context of the recently created MKRCA, the actions of both Maijuna and non-Maijuna individuals in the Sucusari community can have profound consequences on the primate populations of the area. Therefore, the effective conservation of the MKRCA and its primates will depend on the capacity of individuals from different backgrounds and cultural traditions to successfully work together. More specifically, the success of primate conservation will hinge on their willingness to make decisions that have positive, beneficial, and lasting impacts on both forest protection and livelihoods.

Managing hunting in the MKRCA should be community-based and community-driven. Thus, ensuring full participation and involvement of all community members is absolutely critical. Otherwise, any restrictions on the hunting of primates may not be welcome nor completely adhered to. In short, given the biological and cultural value of the MKRCA, the main role of outside stakeholders (e.g., scientists, conservation organizations, and government agencies) should be to help empower the Maijuna and other residents of their communities, through scientific knowledge and monitoring techniques (e.g., hunting registers, biological surveys, etc.), to implement community-based management plans that not only support the conservation goals of the protected area but also their cultural and economic needs. In conclusion, this study stresses the need of integrating an ethnoprimatological approach in primate conservation; ultimately integrating the goals of biodiversity conservation with the cultural and economic needs of indigenous and local communities.

Notes

  1. Mestizos are individuals of mixed Amerindian and Iberian descent who live throughout the Peruvian Amazon region and practice a mixture of traditional agriculture, hunting, fishing, and forest product extraction for their livelihoods [75].

Abbreviations

MKRCA:

Maijuna-Kichwa Regional Conservation Area

References

  1. Estrada A, Garber PA, Rylands AB, Roos C, Fernandez-Duque E, Di Fiore A, Anne-Isola Nekaris K, Nijman V, Heymann EW, Lambert JE, Rovero F, Barelli C, Setchell JM, Gillespie TR, Mittermeier RA, Arregoitia LV, de Guinea M, Gouveia S, Dobrovolski R, Shanee S, Shanee N, Boyle SA, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Amato KR, Meyer ALS, Wich S, Sussman RW, Pan R, Kone I, Li B. Impending extinction crisis of the world’s primates: why primates matter. Sci Adv. 2017;3:e1600946.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Peres CA. Primate community structure at twenty western Amazonian flooded and unflooded forests. J Trop Ecol. 1997;13:381–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Garber PA, Estrada A, Bicca-Marques JC, Heymann EW, Strier KB. South American primates: comparative perspectives in the study of behavior, ecology, and conservation. New York: Springer-Verlag New York; 2009.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  4. Hawes JE, Peres CA. Ecological correlates of trophic status and frugivory in Neotropical primates. Oikos. 2014;123:365–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Bourliere F. Primate communities: their structure and role in tropical ecosystems. Int J Primatol. 1985;6:1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Stevenson PR. Seed dispersal by woolly monkeys (Lagothrix lagotricha) at Tinigua National Park, Colombia: dispersal distance, germination rates, and dispersal quantity. Am J Primatol. 2000;50:275–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Andresen E. Primate seed dispersal by red howler monkeys and the effect of defecation patterns on the fate of dispersed seeds. Biotropica. 2002;34:261–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Wehncke EV, Hubbell SP, Foster RB, Dalling JW. Seed dispersal patterns produced by white-faced monkeys: implications for the dispersal limitation of Neotropical tree species. J Ecol. 2003;91:677–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Laurance WF, Williamson GB. Positive feedbacks among forest fragmentation, drought, and climate change in the Amazon. Conserv Biol. 2001;15:1529–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Fearnside PM. Avança Brasil: environmental and social consequences of Brazil’s planned infrastructure in Amazonia. Environ Manag. 2002;30:735–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Nepstad DC, Stickler CM, Almeida OT. Globalization of the Amazon soy and beef industries: opportunities for conservation. Conserv Biol. 2006;20:1595–603.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Swenson JJ, Carter CE, Domec JC, Delgado CI. Gold mining in the Peruvian Amazon: global prices, deforestation, and mercury imports. PLoS One. 2011; https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018875.

  13. Mittermeier RA. Effects of hunting on rain forest primates. In: Marsh CV, Mittermeier RA, editors. Primate conservation in the tropical rain forest. New York: Alan R. Liss; 1987. p. 109–46.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Peres CA. Effects of hunting on western Amazonian primate communities. Biol Conserv. 1990;54:47–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Peres CA. Effects of subsistence hunting and forest types on the structure of Amazonian primate communities. In: Fleagle JG, Janson C, Reed K, editors. Primate communities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999. p. 268–83.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  16. Ohl-Schacherer J, Shepard GH, Kaplan H, Peres CA, Levi T, Yu DW. The sustainability of subsistence hunting by Matsigenka native communities in Manu National Park, Peru. Conserv Biol. 2007;21:1174–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Levi T, Shepard GH, Ohl-Schacherer J, Peres C, Yu DW. Modelling the long-term sustainability of indigenous hunting in Manu National Park, Peru: landscape-scale management implications for Amazonia. J Appl Ecol. 2009;46:804–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Lizarralde M. Ethnoecology of monkeys among the Barí of Venezuela: perception, use and conservation. In: Fuentes A, Wolfe LD, editors. Primates face to face: the conservation implications of human-nonhuman primates interconnections. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002. p. 85–100.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  19. Shepard GH. Primates in Matsigenka subsistence and world view. In: Fuentes A, Wolfe LD, editors. Primates face to face: the conservation implications of human-nonhuman primates interconnections. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002. p. 101–36.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  20. Cormier LA. Monkey as food, monkey as child: Guajá symbolic cannibalism. In: Fuentes A, Wolfe LD, editors. Primates face to face: the conservation implications of human-nonhuman primates interconnections. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002. p. 63–84.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  21. Papworth S, Milner-Gulland EJ, Slocombe K. The natural place to begin: the ethnoprimatology of the Waorani. Am J Primatol. 2013;75:1117–28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Alves RRN, Souto WMS. Ethnozoology: a brief introduction. Ethnobiol Conserv. 2015;4:1.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Fuentes A, Hockings KJ. The ethnoprimatological approach in primatology. Am J Primatol. 2010;72:841–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. da Silva MNF, Shepard GH, Yu DW. Conservation implications of primate hunting practices among the Matsigenka of Manu National Park. Neotrop Primates. 2005;13:31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Cormier LA. A preliminary review of Neotropical primates in the subsistence and symbolism of indigenous lowland South American peoples. Ecol Environ Anthropol. 2006;2:14–32.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Gilmore MP. An ethnoecological and ethnobotanical study of the Maijuna Indians of the Peruvian Amazon. Miami University, Department of Botany; 2005. PhD thesis.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Gilmore MP, Eshbaugh WH, Greenberg AM. The use, construction and importance of canoes among the Maijuna of the Peruvian Amazon. Econ Bot. 2002;56:10–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Gilmore MP, Endress BA, Horn CM. The socio-cultural importance of Mauritia flexuosa palm swamps (aguajales) and implications for multi-use management in two Maijuna communities of the Peruvian Amazon. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2013;9:29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Gilmore MP. The Maijuna: past, present, and future. In: Gilmore MP, Vriesendorp C, Alverson WS, del Campo Á, Von May R, López Wong C, Ríos Ochoa S, editors. Perú: Maijuna, Rapid biological and social inventories Report 22. Chicago: The Field Museum; 2010. p. 226–33.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Gilmore MP, Vriesendorp C, Alverson WS, del Campo Á, Von May R, López Wong C, Ríos OS. Perú: Maijuna, Rapid biological and social inventories Report 22. Chicago: The Field Museum; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Horn CM, Gilmore MP, Endress BA. Ecological and socio-economic factors influencing aguaje (Mauritia flexuosa) resource management in two indigenous communities in the Peruvian Amazon. For Ecol Manag. 2012;267:93–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. El Peruano. Establecen área de conservación regional Maijuna Kichwa, ubicada en el departamento de Loreto. Decreto No. 008-2015. 2015. http://www.elperuano.com.pe/NormasElperuano/2015/06/17/1252025-2.html. Accessed 27 Dec 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Castner JL. Explorama’s Amazon: a journey through the rainforest of Peru. Gainesville: Feline Press; 2000. p. 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Aquino R, Encarnación F. Primates of Peru/Los Primates del Perú. Primate Rep. 1994;40:1–127.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Aquino R, López L, Arévalo I, Daza J. Diversidad y abundancia de primates y sus amenazas en el interfluvio de los ríos Napo y Putumayo, Amazonía peruana. Rev Peru Biol. 2016;23:243–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Mere C. Comparing traditional ecological knowledge and scientific census data on primate populations in the Sucusari community, Peruvian Amazon. George Mason University, Department of Environmental Science and Policy; 2016. MS thesis.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Gilmore MP, Eshbaugh WH. From researcher to partner: ethical challenges and issues facing the ethnobiological researcher. In: Anderson EN, Pearsall D, Hunn E, Turner N, editors. Ethnobiology. Hoboken: Wiley; 2011. p. 51–63.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  38. Bernard HR. Research methods in anthropology: qualitative and quantitative approaches. 5th ed. Lanham: AltaMira Press; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Rylands AB, Heymann EW, Lynch Alfaro J, Buckner JC, Roos C, Matauschek C, Boubli JP, Sampaio R, Mittermeier RA. Taxonomic review of the new world tamarins (primates: Callitrichidae). Zoo J Linnean Soc. 2016;177:1003–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Defler TR, Bueno ML. Aotus diversity and the species problem. Primate Conserv. 2007;22:55–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. van Roosmalen MG, van Roosmalen T, Mittermeier RA. A taxonomic review of the Titi monkeys, genus Callicebus Thomas, 1903, with the description of two new species Callicebus bernhardi and Callicebus stephennashi, from Brazilian Amazonia. Neotrop Primates. 2002;10:1–52.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Groves CP. Primate taxonomy. Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution Press; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Hershkovitz P. Uacaries, new world monkeys of the genus Cacajao (Cebidae, Plathyrrini): a preliminary taxonomy review with the description of a new subspecies. Am J Primatol. 1987;12:1–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Nieves M, Ascunce MS, Rahn MI, Mudry MD. Phylogenetic relationships among some Ateles species: the use of chromosomic and molecular characters. Primates. 2005;46:155–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Marsh LK. A taxonomic revision of the saki monkeys, Pithecia Desmarest, 1804. Neotrop Primates. 2014;21:82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Lynch Alfaro JW, Silva JS, Rylands AB. How different are robust and gracile Capuchin Monkeys? An argument for the use of Sapajus and Cebus. Am J Primatol. 2012;74:273–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Renoux F, de Thoisy B. Hunting management: the need to adjust predictive models to field observations. Ethnobiol Conserv. 2016;5:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Quinlan M. Considerations for collecting freelists in the field: examples from ethnobotany. Field Methods. 2005;17:219–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Redford KH. The empty forest. Bioscience. 2014;42:412–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Terborgh J. The fate of tropical forests: a matter of stewardship. Conserv Biol. 2000;14:1358–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Alvard MS, Robinson JG, Redford KH, Kaplan H. The sustainability of subsistence hunting in the Neotropics. Conserv Biol. 1997;11:977–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Mesquita GP, Barreto LN. Evaluation of mammals hunting in indigenous and rural localities in Eastern Brazilian Amazon. Ethnobiol Conserv. 2015;4:2.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Urbani B. A survey of primate populations in northeastern Venezuelan Guayana. Primate Conserv. 2006;20:47–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Stafford CA, Alarcon-Valenzuela J, Patiño J, Preziosi RF, Sellers WI. Know your monkey: identifying primate conservation challenges in an indigenous Kichwa community using an ethnoprimatological approach. Folia Primatol. 2016;87:31–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Rosenberger AL, Hartwig WC. New world monkeys. In: Encyclopedia of life sciences: Nature Publishing Group. http://pages.nycep.org/rosenberger/downloads/pdf/NWM_Rosenberger_Hartwig.pdf. Accessed 4 May 2016.

  56. Dougherty JWD. Salience and relativity in classification. Am Ethnol. 1978;5:66–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Chapman CA, Peres CA. Primate conservation in the new millennium: the role of scientists. Evol Anthropol. 2001;10:16–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Di Fiore A, Link A, Campbell CJ. The atelines: behavioral and socioecological diversity in a new world radiation. In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Panger M, Beader SK, editors. Primates in perspective. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. p. 155–88.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Bodmer RE, Eisenberg JF, Redford KH. Hunting and the likelihood of extinction of Amazonian mammals. Conserv Biol. 1997;11:460–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Voss RS, Fleck DW. Mammalian diversity and Matsés ethnomammalogy in Amazonian Peru part 1: primates. Bull Am Mus Nat Hist. 2011;351:1–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Redford KH, Robinson JG. Hunting by indigenous peoples and conservation of game species. Cult Surviv Q. 1985;9:41–4.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Van Vliet N, Moreno J, Gómez J, Zhou W, Fa JE, Golden C, Nóbrega-Alves RR, Nasi R. Bushmeat and human health: assessing the evidence in tropical and subtropical forests. Ethnobiol Conserv. 2017;6:3.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Hotez PJ, Bottazzi ME, Franco-Paredes C, Ault SK, Periago MR. The neglected tropical diseases of Latin America and the Caribbean: a review of disease burden and distribution and a roadmap for control and elimination. PLoS Negl Trop Dis. 2008;2(9):e300. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pntd.0000300

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Erkenswick GA, Watsa M, Pacheco MA, Escalante AA, Parker PG. Chronic plasmodium infections in wild Peruvian tamarins. PLoS One. 2017;12(9):e0184504. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184504

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Dallam HE. The growing voice of indigenous peoples: their use of storytelling and rights discourse to transform multilateral development bank policies. Ariz J Int’l Comp L. 1991;8:117–48.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Martin GJ. Ethnobotany: a methods manual (people and plant’s conservation manual). London: Chapman and Hall; 1995.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  67. Bennett D. Stepping from the diagram: Australian aboriginal cultural and spiritual values relating to biodiversity. In: Posey DA, editor. Cultural and spiritual values of biodiversity. London: UNEP and Intermediate Technology Publications; 1999. p. 103–5.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Posey DA, Dutfield G. Beyond intellectual property: toward traditional resource rights for indigenous peoples and local communities. Otawa: International Development Research Centre; 1996. p. 103–5.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Nakashima D, Roué M. Indigenous knowledge, peoples and sustainable practice. Encycl Glob Environ Chang. 2002;5:314–24.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Reyes-García V, Paneque-Gálves J, Luz A, Gueze M, Macía M, Orta-Martínez M, Pino J. Cultural change in traditional ecological knowledge: an empirical analysis from the Tsimane’ in the Bolivian Amazon. Hum Organ. 2014;73:162–73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Redford KH, Stearman AM. Forest-dwelling native Amazonians and the conservation of biodiversity: interests in common or in collision? Conserv Biol. 1993;7:248–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Charnley S, Fischer AP, Jones ET. Integrating traditional and local ecological knowledge into forest biodiversity conservation in the Pacific Northwest. For Ecol Manag. 2007;246:14–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Prado HM, Murrieta RSS, Adams C, Brondizio ES. Local and scientific knowledge for assessing the use of fallows and mature forest by large mammals in SE Brazil: identifying singularities in folkecology. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2014;10:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Parry L, Peres CA. Evaluating the use of local ecological knowledge to monitor hunted tropical-forest wildlife over large spatial scales. Ecol Soc. 2015;20:15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Coomes OT, Ban N. Cultivated plant species diversity in home gardens of an Amazonian peasant village in northeastern Peru. Econ Bot. 2004;58:420–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Emmons LH, Feer F. Neotropical rainforest mammals: a field guide. 2nd ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Velie D. Vocabulario Orejón. Serie Lingüística Peruana, No. 16. Instituto Lingüístico de Verano: Pucallpa; 1981.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are profoundly grateful to the Sucusari community for their willingness to share their knowledge with us. A special thanks to Sebastián Ríos Ochoa for his commitment and support throughout the entire research project. We would also like to thank Dr. Elizabeth Freeman for her suggestions that helped to improve this project. Publication of this article was funded in part by the George Mason University Libraries Open Access Publishing Fund.

Funding

Not applicable

Availability of data and materials

All available data from this study are included in this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MPG, MB, and CMR conceptualized the study and designed the questionnaire. CMR conducted data collection and analysis as well as wrote the manuscript. All authors have read, edited, and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carla Mere Roncal.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

Research was conducted with the approval of the Maijuna community of Sucusari as well as the George Mason University Office of Research Integrity and Assurance (ORIA), reference number 591846-1. Before conducting the interviews, prior informed consent was obtained from the community as well as research participants.

Consent for publication

All data were published with the prior informed consent of research participants.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1

English translation of the traditional Maijuna monkey creation story. Story told by Samuel Ríos Flores, a master Maijuna storyteller. The Maijuna version of this story is presented in Appendix 2. The numbered sentences in the English version of this story correspond exactly to the Maijuna version.

1“We want to eat fruits,” [said the group of people]. 2“You want to eat fruits?” [asked the Creator]. 3“Yes, we wish we could go up and eat them,” [replied the group of people]. 4“If you want fruits then untie the rope from your hammock and place it down toward your butt (to make a tail),” [said the Creator]. 5[In the meantime] the Creator was grating Genipa americana fruits. 6After grating the fruit he rubbed it on their faces and mouths (Genipa americana fruits are used to make a black dye and, according to Maijuna traditions, this is why Lagothrix lagotricha monkeys have black faces.) 7“Who wants to be a naso (L. lagotricha)?” [he asked]. 8“I do,” [a woman replied]. 9“What does a naso (L. lagotricha) sound like while eating fruits?” [asked the Creator]. 10She listened to him, climbed a tree, and said: “chichi, chichi, chichi.” 11“You are a chichi (L. nigricollis). 12You are not a real naso (L. lagotricha),” he said. 13“This is what I will be then,” the women said. (At this moment she became a chichi monkey.) 14“Who is going to be a naso (L. lagotricha)?” [the Creator asked again]. 15“Me,” [replied a man]. 16“What does a real naso (L. lagotricha) sound like?” [asked the Creator]. 17The man climbed a tree and happily gathered fruits. 18[He then called out], “Choyoro, choyoro, choyoro.” 19“Yes, you are a naso (L. lagotricha),” [said the Creator]. 20“Now, who will be a jaiquɨ (Alouatta seniculus)?” [asked the Creator]. 21“Me. 22I want to be a jaiquɨ (A. seniculus),” [a women replied]. 23“Let me hear you. 24Sing so I can hear how a jaiquɨ (A. seniculus) sings,” [requested the Creator]. 25“Oju, oju, oju, oju,” [she sang]. 26“You are a bao (Callicebus lucifer)”, [said the Creator]. 27“This is what I will be then,” [replied the woman]. 28“Now, who will be a jaiquɨ (A. seniculus)?” [asked the Creator]. 29“Me,” [a man replied]. 30The creator then placed a small bichibɨ (gourd) into the man’s throat. 31Then, the creator rubbed annatto over his entire body (to paint it red). 32“Sing so I can hear if you are really a jaiquɨ (A. seniculus). 33How will you sing when it rains?” [asked the Creator]. 34“Ogu, ogu, ogu, oguuu,” [he howled]. 35“Yes, you are a jaiquɨ (A. seniculus),” [said the Creator]. 36The end.

Appendix 2

Maijuna version of the traditional Maijuna monkey creation story. Story told by Samuel Ríos Flores, a master Maijuna storyteller.

1Acue acueyo oiyi yiquɨa. 2Mɨsa acue oiye. 3Quima mɨni acueyo oiyi. 4Acue oijɨ ani mɨsa jaɨoma josema mɨsajuna bari oje tatecachi. 5Be ɨrɨguɨ. 6Be ɨrɨre yia quɨrɨguɨ yobɨ tea. 7Nebɨ naso bayo ijɨ. 8Yi bachi ico. 9Quima jɨcaquɨ naso acue acuequɨ ani. 10Asare mɨico chi, chi, chi, chi ico. 11Chichina ja. 12Aje nasona ja ijɨ. 13Cao ñi bachi ico. 14Nebɨ bayo. 15Yia ijɨ. 16Quima jɨcaquɨ naso debɨ ani. 17Mɨni acue tɨtequɨ chibajɨ. 18Choyoro, choyoro, choyoro. 19Ase cta nasona chibajɨ. 20Jana igueca ne bayo jaiquɨ. 21Yia ico. 22Yi jaiquɨ bachi. 23Ja jɨcama asayi. 24Ja yima asayi jaiquɨ quima yiquɨ. 25Oju, oju, oju, oju. 26Baonata. 27Caoñi bachi ico. 28Jana igueca ne bayo jaiquɨ. 29Yia ijɨ. 30Bichibɨ tatecaquɨ. 31Bosa socaquɨ. 32Ja yima asayi jaiquɨ ani. 33Ocotu quima yiquɨ bachi. 34Ogu, ogu, ogu, oguuu. 35Ase cta jaiquɨna ja chibajɨ. 36Casoa ja.

Appendix 3

English translation of the traditional Maijuna story of the red titi monkey (Callicebus discolor). Story told by Romero Ríos Ochoa.

There was a lazy man, who never wanted to work or walk. For him it was a problem and he didn’t know what was a matter. A wise person came and told him, “You have some animals in your body.” The man asked, “How can we kill these animals?”

“We are going to offer them fruits (Annona spp.) so that they come down to eat. We will kill them when they come down to eat and you will become a hard-working person,” answered the wise man. When the fruits were set down, monkeys descended from the man’s shoulders and started to eat the fruits. However, one monkey didn’t go down, it stayed inside the man’s body. The rest of the monkeys were killed by the man.

With fewer burdens on his body, the man began to work and he was no longer lazy. All of these animals were ñame bao (Callicebus discolor), for this reason they are called the “lazy monkeys.” The end.

Appendix 4

English translation of the traditional Maijuna story of the common squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). Story told by Felipe Navarro Rojas.

The people were looking at the trees (Inga spp.). They really wanted to eat the fruits. They asked themselves, “How can we reach the fruits?” At this moment, Maineno appeared. “What are you talking about?” asked Maineno. “We want to eat the fruits but we are not able to”, replied the group of people. Maineno then let them climb the trees and said, “Now you will all be bo chichi (Saimiri sciureus).” That is how bo chichi were created.

Appendix 5

English translation of the traditional Kichwa story of the red titi monkey (Callicebus discolor) and red howler monkey (Alouatta seniculus). Story told by Orlando Coquinche Ruiz.

The red titi monkey (Callicebus discolor) had its little drum (referring to the modified hyoid bone used for vocalization) similar to the red howler monkey (Alouatta seniculus). The drum of the red titi monkey was louder [than that of the red howler monkey]. One day, the red titi monkey lent his drum to the red howler monkey because the howler’s was not producing a strong sound. After lending his drum to the red howler monkey, the red howler monkey never gave it back. For this reason, the red titi monkey no longer sings loud and now the red howler monkey beats it at singing and sings much louder.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mere Roncal, C., Bowler, M. & Gilmore, M.P. The ethnoprimatology of the Maijuna of the Peruvian Amazon and implications for primate conservation. J Ethnobiology Ethnomedicine 14, 19 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-018-0207-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-018-0207-x

Keywords